+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda …FULL LENGTH ARTICLE Is the phonotactics of the...

Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda …FULL LENGTH ARTICLE Is the phonotactics of the...

Date post: 17-Jan-2020
Category:
Upload: others
View: 5 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
13
FULL LENGTH ARTICLE Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-based? Yasser A.S. Al Tamimi a, * , Yousef Al Shboul b a Alfaisal University, Riyadh, Saudi Arabia b Hashemite University, Zarqa, Jordan Received 1 June 2012; accepted 4 June 2012 KEYWORDS Sonority Sequencing Princi- ple; Modern Standard Arabic; CVCC syllable; Conformity; Sonority reversals; Sonority plateaus Abstract The study investigates the phonotactics of coda consonant clusters in Modern Standard Arabic CVCC syllable from the perspective of the Sonority Sequencing Principle (SSP). Based on around 500 CVCC lexical items listed in The Hans Wehr Dictionary of Modern Written Arabic, and on Hogg and McCully’s (1987) sonority scale, the study provides an exhaustive quantitative account of all possible coda manifestations which have been found in this study to fall into three major categories: conformity (42%), sonority reversals (49%), and sonority plateaus (9%). The study specifies and thoroughly exemplifies the patterns and subpatterns under each, and concludes, given the 58% of violation, that SSP is not a reliable phonological predictor for the sequencing of the consonant clusters in Modern Standard Arabic CVCC coda, contrary to long standing phono- logical assumptions that put much weight on the explanatory adequacy of this principle. ª 2013 King Saud University. Production and hosting by Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction Segments are said to be organized into well-formed sequences according to universal principles. Whether within the syllable or across syllables, this sequencing is traditionally held to be driven by principles of sonority, a property that ranks seg- ments a long a hierarchy from most sonorous to least sono- rous. Amongst such principles is the Sonority Sequencing Principle (SSP), which stipulates that onsets rise in sonority toward the nucleus, while codas fall in sonority. (Clements, 1990; Parker, 2002). SSP is claimed to account for strong cross-linguistic distri- butional and sequential tendencies (Geirut, 1999), and differ- ent syllable structures in different languages have been studied within its framework since Sievers (1881). Whether this claim is true or not for Modern Standard Arabic (MSA) coda in the extra heavy CVCC# syllable is a question that has re- mained, to our knowledge, unsatisfactorily answered, as the available literature, if not impressionistic, lacks for the most part a thorough and an exhaustive investigation based on quantitative evidence. The general indication one can obtain from this literature is that Modern Standard Arabic CC coda normally conforms to the principle, and if it does not, a vowel epenthetic rule applies (See Section 3 below). This generaliza- tion, though insightful, leaves much uncertainty about the ex- tent to which the phonotactics of the coda clusters is canonical from the perspective of the Sonority Sequencing Principle. It is * Corresponding author. Tel.: +966 561252200; fax: +966 12157730. E-mail addresses: [email protected], yaltamimi@alfaisa- l.edu (Y.A.S. Al Tamimi), [email protected] (Y. Al Shboul). Peer review under responsibility of King Saud University. Production and hosting by Elsevier Journal of King Saud University – Languages and Translation (2013) xxx, xxxxxx King Saud University Journal of King Saud University – Languages and Translation www.ksu.edu.sa www.sciencedirect.com 2210-8319 ª 2013 King Saud University. Production and hosting by Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jksult.2012.12.003 Please cite this article in press as: Al Tamimi, Y.A.S., Al Shboul, Y. Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority- based?. Journal of King Saud University – Languages and Translation (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jksult.2012.12.003
Transcript
Page 1: Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda …FULL LENGTH ARTICLE Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-based? Yasser A.S. Al Tamimi a,*, Yousef Al Shboul b a

Journal of King Saud University – Languages and Translation (2013) xxx, xxx–xxx

King Saud University

Journal of King Saud University –

Languages and Translationwww.ksu.edu.sa

www.sciencedirect.com

FULL LENGTH ARTICLE

Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda

sonority-based?

Yasser A.S. Al Tamimi a,*, Yousef Al Shboul b

a Alfaisal University, Riyadh, Saudi Arabiab Hashemite University, Zarqa, Jordan

Received 1 June 2012; accepted 4 June 2012

*

12E

l.e

Pe

22

ht

Pb

KEYWORDS

Sonority Sequencing Princi-

ple;

Modern Standard Arabic;

CVCC syllable;

Conformity;

Sonority reversals;

Sonority plateaus

Corresponding author. T

157730.-mail addresses: yaseraltam

du (Y.A.S. Al Tamimi), yshb

er review under responsibilit

Production an

10-8319 ª 2013 King Saud U

tp://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jksu

lease cite this article in prased?. Journal of King Sa

el.: +9

imi@hotm

ool@hu

y of King

d hostin

niversity

lt.2012.12

ess as: Aud Univ

Abstract The study investigates the phonotactics of coda consonant clusters in Modern Standard

Arabic CVCC syllable from the perspective of the Sonority Sequencing Principle (SSP). Based on

around 500 CVCC lexical items listed in The Hans Wehr Dictionary of Modern Written Arabic,

and on Hogg and McCully’s (1987) sonority scale, the study provides an exhaustive quantitative

account of all possible coda manifestations which have been found in this study to fall into three

major categories: conformity (42%), sonority reversals (49%), and sonority plateaus (9%). The

study specifies and thoroughly exemplifies the patterns and subpatterns under each, and concludes,

given the 58% of violation, that SSP is not a reliable phonological predictor for the sequencing of

the consonant clusters in Modern Standard Arabic CVCC coda, contrary to long standing phono-

logical assumptions that put much weight on the explanatory adequacy of this principle.ª 2013 King Saud University. Production and hosting by Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction

Segments are said to be organized into well-formed sequencesaccording to universal principles. Whether within the syllableor across syllables, this sequencing is traditionally held to be

driven by principles of sonority, a property that ranks seg-ments a long a hierarchy from most sonorous to least sono-rous. Amongst such principles is the Sonority Sequencing

Principle (SSP), which stipulates that onsets rise in sonority

66 561252200; fax: +966

ail.com, yaltamimi@alfaisa-

.edu.jo (Y. Al Shboul).

Saud University.

g by Elsevier

. Production and hosting by Elsev

.003

l Tamimi, Y.A.S., Al Shboul,ersity – Languages and Transl

toward the nucleus, while codas fall in sonority. (Clements,

1990; Parker, 2002).SSP is claimed to account for strong cross-linguistic distri-

butional and sequential tendencies (Geirut, 1999), and differ-ent syllable structures in different languages have been

studied within its framework since Sievers (1881). Whether thisclaim is true or not for Modern Standard Arabic (MSA) codain the extra heavy CVCC# syllable is a question that has re-

mained, to our knowledge, unsatisfactorily answered, as theavailable literature, if not impressionistic, lacks for the mostpart a thorough and an exhaustive investigation based on

quantitative evidence. The general indication one can obtainfrom this literature is that Modern Standard Arabic CC codanormally conforms to the principle, and if it does not, a vowel

epenthetic rule applies (See Section 3 below). This generaliza-tion, though insightful, leaves much uncertainty about the ex-tent to which the phonotactics of the coda clusters is canonicalfrom the perspective of the Sonority Sequencing Principle. It is

ier B.V. All rights reserved.

Y. Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-ation (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jksult.2012.12.003

Page 2: Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda …FULL LENGTH ARTICLE Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-based? Yasser A.S. Al Tamimi a,*, Yousef Al Shboul b a

2 Y.A.S. Al Tamimi, Y. Al Shboul

the endeavor of the present descriptive and quantitative re-search to bridge this gap in the literature.

The findings of a pilot study we performed revealed many

incidence of CC clusters that can be categorized under whatmight be called ‘‘sonority reversals’’ and ‘‘plateaus’’ (Morellie,1999: 20, Section 2.2 below), in addition to conformity, which

encouraged us to widen the scope of the data of the presentstudy to include all the CVCC lexical items listed in TheHans-Wehr Dictionary of Modern Written Arabic – around

500 words in number.Based on this exhaustive dictionary-based data, and adopt-

ing Hogg and McCully (1987) sonority scale for relevance (seeSection 2 below), the study aims to thoroughly and quantita-

tively answer the following questions related to the CC clustersof Modern Standard Arabic CVCC lexical items:

1. What are the coda clusters that conform to the SonoritySequencing principle? What are their different patternsand subpatterns? How frequent is each?

2. What are the coda clusters that demonstrate sonority rever-sals? What are their different patterns and subpatterns?How frequent is each?

3. What are the coda clusters that exhibit sonority plateaus?What are their different patterns and subpatterns? How fre-quent is each?

4. In view of the findings, is the Sonority Sequencing Principle

a reliable phonological predictor for Modern StandardArabic complex coda?

Given the purely descriptive and quantitative approach itadopts and the ample evidence it provides, the study is meantto be a detailed reference for researchers on the sonority of

Arabic complex coda in CVCC syllables.

Table 2 Hogg and McCully’s Sonority Scale (1987).

Sound Sonority value Sound So

Low vowels 10 Flaps 7

Mid vowels 9 Laterals 6

High vowels 8 Nasals 5

Table 1 Modern standard Arabic consonant invent

Please cite this article in press as: Al Tamimi, Y.A.S., Al Shboul,based?. Journal of King Saud University – Languages and Trans

2. Background

2.1. Modern Standard Arabic

Modern Standard Arabic is the standard form of the Arabiccontemporary era, and the written record of its modern culture

(Al Soswah, 2002). Presented in Table 1 below is the consonantinventory of this standard, as adapted from Amayreh (2003).

Modern Standard Arabic has a simple vowel system: three

short monophthongs: open /a/, close back /u/, and close front /i/, and their long equivalents /a:/, /u: / and / i:/, respectively (AlOtaibi and Hussain, 2010), and its syllable types include CV,CVV, CVC, CVVC, CVCC (our emphasis), and CVVCC

(Holes, 2004), where C and V represent a consonant and a vo-wel, respectively. Its syllable is similar to that of English inhaving a nucleus (an obligatory segment either short or long),

onset, and an optional coda, but different in allowing no morethan one consonant in the onset.

2.2. Sonority Sequencing Principle

Complex onsets and codas are claimed to be governed by theSonority Sequencing Principle (SPS), which suggests, as stated

above, that sonority increases monotonically the closer onegets to the sonority peak (the nucleus), and decreases as onegets away from that peak (Goldsmith, 1990: 110). Since sonor-ity can be best defined in terms of intensity, in addition to air-

flow obstruction and voice (Ladefoged, 1993; Moreton et al.,2008), preference in sonority research has been given to thesonority scales which are supported by intensity measurement,

such as those put forward by Kiparsky (1979), Hogg andMcCully (1987), Clements (1990) and Parker (2002). However,in addition, Hogg and McCully’s scale, shown in Table 2

nority value Sound Sonority value

Voiced fricative 4

Voiceless fricative 3

Voiced stops 2

Voiceless stops 1

ory.

Y. Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-lation (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jksult.2012.12.003

Page 3: Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda …FULL LENGTH ARTICLE Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-based? Yasser A.S. Al Tamimi a,*, Yousef Al Shboul b a

Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-based? 3

above, is known for its relative accuracy characterized byestablishing sonority differences amongst different vowel clas-ses (i.e. low, mid, high), and by breaking down obstruents into

fricatives and stops, and these, in turn, into voiced and voice-less. This specification suits the meticulous nature of thedescriptive approach adopted in the present study, and makes

possible answering the research questions.According to Carlisle (2001): (4–5), ‘‘though the Sonority

Sequencing Principle expresses a very strong universal tendency,

complex margins may violate it in two manners. First, two seg-ments in a margin may have the same sonority; these are knownas sonority plateaus (Clements, 1990) and are found in a few lan-guages, including English, as in the words ‘sphere’ and ‘fact’.

Second, themore peripheral in the onset or codamay have high-er sonority than a segment closer to the nucleus, such aberrantsonority profiles are known as reversals and occur in some lan-

guages including English as exemplified by spin, sky, ax, hops’’.

3. Review of related literature

The phonotactics of Modern Standard Arabic consonant clus-ters in the CVCC syllable has been handled in a few studies ofdifferent phonological interests. For instance, in his study of

the role of h in repairing syllable structures, Obeidat (2010)handles the CVCC syllable, considering its complex coda ‘‘dif-ficult to pronounce’’ in some instances, including /s¤abr/ ‘pa-

tience’, /�hid3l/ ‘anklet’ and /nuqr/ ‘sound’. Such codas, headds, were even variably epenthesized by vowels in ClassicalArabic as he traced in Amr bin Al’alaa’s Reading of the HolyQuran; in the poetry of Al Rajiz, and in Tamim Arabic. In the

same vein, Kenstowics (1986), in his ‘‘Notes on Syllable Struc-ture’’, presents a set of CVCC lexical items representing Bed-ouin Jordanian Arabic, though many of which sound

standard, and concludes that the Sonority Sequencing Princi-ple can operate on the codas of /dars/, ‘lesson’, and /bint/,‘girl’, but not on that of /�himl/, ‘load’, realized consequently

as [�himil]. Since the ml coda does not demonstrate ‘‘a fallingsonority profile’’ (Kenstowics, p. 120), as stipulated by theprinciple, it is vowel-epenthesized. The indication one may ob-

tain from such studies is that it is the norm for the coda con-sonant clusters to conform to the principle, and if it does not insome instances, vowel epenthesis regularly applies. In this con-text, it is worth citing Labov (1966: 49) who contends that

‘‘such terms as ‘regularly’, ‘rarely’ and ‘sometimes’, etc. areof limited value in a scientific discipline and a quantitativetreatment based on frequency of occurrence is called for’’.

The same indication can be obtained from the abundant re-search on the subject matter in different non-standard dialects(e.g., Haddad, 1983; Mansour, 1991; Kiparsky, 2003; Btoosh,

2006; Al Jumah, 2008; Daana, 2009). However, research basedon quantitative evidence, which is comparatively few, providesmore specific conclusions. Watson (2002), for instance, findsthat 30% of the San’ani Arabic complex coda exhibits sonority

reversals, and far less incidence of sonority plateaus. Similarconclusions were drawn for Modern Standard Hebrew (e.g.Gishri, 2009), a genetically related language.

In brief, despite its insightful observations, the available re-search provides no exhaustive and quantitative account ofModern Standard Arabic coda clusters that conform to SSP,

or violate it in the manner of sonority reversals and plateaus.It neither identifies the different patterns and sub patterns

Please cite this article in press as: Al Tamimi, Y.A.S., Al Shboul,based?. Journal of King Saud University – Languages and Transl

under each category, nor does it provide their frequency ofoccurrence. This constitutes the rationale behind conductingthe present research, which attempts to bridge these gaps by

answering the research questions stated in section 1.

4. Methodology

4.1. Data collection

Dictionary-based data can be used in sonority research. Kam-busziya and Serish (2006), for instance, based their analysis ofvowel epenthesis in the Persian CVCC syllable on dictionary

data and so did Orzechowska and Wiese (2011) in their inves-tigation of the phonotactics of German initial clusters. Consis-tently, all the CVCC lexical items listed in The Hans-Wehr

Dictionary of Modern Written Arabic were collected and con-sidered in the present study. The 493 words found were eitherdeverbal nouns or underived nouns. According to Kenstowics(1986), ‘‘deverbal nouns are a good place to study the forma-

tion of such syllables since CVCC happens to be the underly-ing canonical shape for a large class of deverbalnominalization. CVCC class is also populated by an equally

large number of basic nonderived nouns’’ (p. 101).One main advantage of this dictionary is that it is ‘‘com-

piled on scientific descriptive principles: only words and

expressions that are attested in context are included’’ (HansWehr and Cowan, 1994: VII). However, as dictionary items,these words appear in their citation or pausal forms. For

example, the deverbal CVCC noun ‘karb’ (misfortune) is apausal form that can be found in the dictionary and can alsobe heard in formal speech if the speaker prefers not to usethe word’s inflectional forms, i.e. ‘karbun’; ‘karban’ or ‘karbin’

(where -un, -an, and -in represent the nominative, accusativeand dative case endings, respectively). In fact, many MSAspeakers, we observe, often use the pausal form even in con-

nected speech in order to avoid some inflectional complexities,and this gives us the legitimacy of considering the CC coda ofthis form. Moreover, it is only in this form that one can

straightforwardly identify the CC clusters of the CVCCsyllable.

4.2. Data analysis

The CVCC lexical items were phonemically transcribed andanalyzed in terms of onset, nucleus, coda first consonant,and coda second consonant in order to encompass each

syllable examined. The coda consonants in their phonemicforms were identified and checked against Hogg andMcCully’s Sonority Scale presented in Table 2 above. Each

coda cluster in each lexical item was categorized where it fitsunder any of the three sonority possibilities: conformity, rever-sals and plateaus, and the patterns and subpatterns of each

category were identified. Consistent with the sonority scaleused, affricates were treated as stop consonants. Coarticula-tion, which may have some influence on consonant clustering(e.g. Jongstra, 2003) has not been considered in this study as

it uses dictionary lexical items in the first place, and it, in thesecond, adopts Hogg and McCully’s sonority scale thatexcludes coarticulatory effects. As such, the study is more

concerned with the coda consonant phonemes rather than withtheir different phonetic realizations. MSA lexical items after all

Y. Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-ation (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jksult.2012.12.003

Page 4: Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda …FULL LENGTH ARTICLE Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-based? Yasser A.S. Al Tamimi a,*, Yousef Al Shboul b a

4 Y.A.S. Al Tamimi, Y. Al Shboul

are typically taken as the underlying representations for theirdifferent phonetic forms that exist in different Arabic dialects.

5. Results and discussion

As checked against Hogg andMcCully’s sonority scale, the con-sonant clusters of the 493 CVCC lexical items appear to fall into

the three major categories: conformity, sonority reversals andsonority plateaus. Conformity has been observed in 208 cases(42%); sonority reversals in 241 (49%); and sonority plateaus

in only 44 instances (9%), as demonstrated in Fig. 1 below.

5.1. Conformity to SSP

As stated above, (42%) of the CC codas appear to conform toSSP. These ‘‘core clusters’’ or the ‘‘unmarked clusters’’ (Morel-lie, 1999: 20) have been found to fall into six major patterns: 1)

flap + consonant, 2) lateral + consonant, 3) nasal + conso-nant, 4) voiced fricative + consonant, 5) voicelessfricative + consonant, and 6) voiced stop + voiceless stop,as exhibited in Fig. 2.

A part from the voiced stop + voiceless stop pattern, eachother conformity patterns has been found to comprise a num-ber of subpatterns, as specified below.

5.1.1. Flap + consonant

Fifty-six CC coda instances out of 208 (27% of the conform-ing codas) were found to follow the pattern flap + conso-

Figure 1 MSA CC coda and SSP.

Figure 2 Conformity patterns.

Please cite this article in press as: Al Tamimi, Y.A.S., Al Shboul,based?. Journal of King Saud University – Languages and Trans

nant, where the second consonant can be nasal (2instances), voiced fricative (2), voiceless fricative (23), voice-less stop (7), voiced stop (20), and glottal stop (2), as shown

in Fig. 3 below.

5.1.2. Lateral + consonant

The lateral + consonant coda pattern has been observed in 27

cases (13%) that can be categorized into 5 subpatterns: 1) la-teral + nasal (3 cases), 2) lateral + voiced fricative (3), 3)lateral + voiceless fricative (11), 4) lateral + voiced stop (5),

and 5) lateral + voiceless stop (5 instances), as demonstratedin Fig. 4 below.

5.1.3. Nasal + consonant

The nasal + consonant coda pattern has been seen in 39 in-stances out of 208 (19%) distributed into four basic subpat-terns: 1) nasal + voiced fricative (11 instances), 2)

nasal + voiceless fricative (17), 3) nasal + voiced stop (8),and 4) nasal + voiceless stop (3), as illustrated in Fig. 5 below.

5.1.4. Voiced fricative + consonant

The voiced fricative + consonant coda pattern has been ob-served in 20 cases (10%) that spread out in three subpatterns:1) voiced fricative + voiced stop (10 instances), 2) voiced fric-

ative + voiceless stop (4), and 3) voiced fricative + voicelessfricative (6 instances), as demonstrated in Fig. 6 below.

5.1.5. Voiceless fricative + consonant

The voiceless fricative + consonant coda pattern has beenseen in 53 cases (25%) unfolding in three subpatterns: 1) voice-less fricative + voiceless stop (25 instances), 2) voiceless frica-

tive + voiced stop (26) and 3) voiceless fricative + glottal stop(2), as shown in Fig. 7 below.

5.1.6. Voiced stop + voiceless stop

Only a voiceless stop can function as a second consonant in theconforming codas whose first consonant is a voiced stop. Thisclustering has been observed in 13 instances out of 208 (6%),

as represented in Fig. 8 below.Having identified the different CC patterns and subpatterns

that conform to SSP, and shown that this conformity can only

appear in 42% of the data (thus answering the first researchquestion), we can conclude that the CC coda of MSA CVCCsyllable is only partially conditioned by this principle, and thisprovides an answer to the forth research question. The results

lend no support to the indications obtained from some studies(e.g. Kenstowics, 1986; Obeidat, 2010) which suggest thatMSA CC coda normally complies with SSP. They also disfa-

vor the conclusions of general conformity to SSP drawn fordialectal Arabic (e.g. Haddad,1983; Kiparsky, 2003; Btoosh,2006; Al Jumah 2008; Daana, 2009). Legitimate MSA CC co-

das that violate SSP in the manner of sonority reversals andplateaus are discussed below.

5.2. Reversals

As mentioned above, almost half of the CC clusters in the datacan be regarded as sonority reversals: 241 cases out of 493(49%). This category has been found to include the following

six patterns, which are the reverse of the conforming patterns:

Y. Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-lation (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jksult.2012.12.003

Page 5: Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda …FULL LENGTH ARTICLE Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-based? Yasser A.S. Al Tamimi a,*, Yousef Al Shboul b a

Figure 3 Conformity of flap + consonant.

Figure 4 Conformity of lateral + consonant.

Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-based? 5

1) consonant + flap, 2) consonant + lateral, 3) conso-nant + nasal, 4) consonant + voiced fricative, 5) conso-

Please cite this article in press as: Al Tamimi, Y.A.S., Al Shboul,based?. Journal of King Saud University – Languages and Transl

nant + voiceless fricative, and 6) voiceless stop + voicedstop, as demonstrated in Fig. 9 below.

Y. Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-ation (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jksult.2012.12.003

Page 6: Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda …FULL LENGTH ARTICLE Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-based? Yasser A.S. Al Tamimi a,*, Yousef Al Shboul b a

Figure 5 Conformity of nasal + consonant.

Figure 6 Conformity of voiced fricative + consonant.

6 Y.A.S. Al Tamimi, Y. Al Shboul

Please cite this article in press as: Al Tamimi, Y.A.S., Al Shboul, Y. Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-based?. Journal of King Saud University – Languages and Translation (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jksult.2012.12.003

Page 7: Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda …FULL LENGTH ARTICLE Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-based? Yasser A.S. Al Tamimi a,*, Yousef Al Shboul b a

Figure 7 Conformity of Voiceless Fricative + Consonant.

Figure 8 Conformity of voiced stop + voiceless stop clusters.

Figure 9 Reversal patterns.

Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-based? 7

5.2.1. Consonant + flap

Fifty-nine CC coda instances out of 241 (2.4.5%) have beenfound to follow the pattern: consonant + flap, where the first

consonant can be: nasal (1 incidence), voiced fricative (6),voiceless fricative (26), voiced stop (10), voiceless stop (14),and glottal stop (2), as shown in Fig. 10 below.

5.2.2. Consonant + lateral

The lateral + consonant coda pattern has been observed in 46CVCC lexical items (19%) that can be categorized into the fol-

Please cite this article in press as: Al Tamimi, Y.A.S., Al Shboul,based?. Journal of King Saud University – Languages and Transl

lowing six subpatterns: 1) nasal + lateral (4 cases), 2) voicedfricative + lateral (8), 3) voiceless fricative + lateral (18

cases), 4) voiced stop + lateral (6). 5) voiceless stop + lateral(8), and 6) glottal stop + lateral (2 cases); as demonstratedin Fig. 11 below.

Y. Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-ation (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jksult.2012.12.003

Page 8: Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda …FULL LENGTH ARTICLE Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-based? Yasser A.S. Al Tamimi a,*, Yousef Al Shboul b a

Figure 11 Sonority reversals of consonant + lateral.

Figure 10 Sonority reversals of consonant + flap.

8 Y.A.S. Al Tamimi, Y. Al Shboul

Please cite this article in press as: Al Tamimi, Y.A.S., Al Shboul, Y. Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-based?. Journal of King Saud University – Languages and Translation (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jksult.2012.12.003

Page 9: Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda …FULL LENGTH ARTICLE Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-based? Yasser A.S. Al Tamimi a,*, Yousef Al Shboul b a

Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-based? 9

5.2.3. Consonant + nasal

The consonant + nasal pattern has been observed in 58 in-

stances out of the 241 (24%) that can be categorized into foursubpatterns: 1) voiced fricative + nasal (13 instances), 2)voiceless fricative + nasal (25), 3) voiced stop + nasal (12),

4) voiceless stop + nasal (6), and 5) glottal stop + nasal (2),as displayed in Fig. 12 below.

5.2.4. Consonant + voiced fricative

The consonant + voiced fricative coda pattern has been seenin 29 cases (12%) that can appear in three subpatterns: 1)voiced stop + voiced fricative (15 instances), 2) voiceless

stop + voiced fricative (7), and 3) voiceless fricative + voicedfricative (7) instances), as shown in Fig. 13 below.

5.2.5. Consonant + voiceless fricative

The consonant + voiceless fricative coda pattern has beenobserved in 39 cases (16%) that can be observed in threesubpatterns: 1) voiceless stop + voiceless fricative (13 cases),

2) voiced stop + voiceless fricative (25) and 3) glottal

Figure 12 Sonority reversa

Please cite this article in press as: Al Tamimi, Y.A.S., Al Shboul,based?. Journal of King Saud University – Languages and Transl

stop + voiceless fricative (one case), as shown in Fig. 14below.

5.2.6. Voiceless stop + V voiced stop

The reversal pattern of voiceless stop + voiced stop has beenseen in only 10 cases out of 241 (4%), as represented inFig. 15 below.

Having identified the different CC patterns and sub pat-terns that exhibit sonority reversals, and shown that this dras-tic violation occurs in 49% of the data (thus answering the

second research question), we can confirm our previous con-clusion that SSP is not a reliable phonological predictor forthe CC coda of MSA CVCC syllable. In principle, the exis-

tence of incidence of sonority reversals reported by Kenstowics(1986), Obeidat (2010) is supported, though quantitativelyrevealed here to be much greater than implied in their indica-

tions. The results also back up Watson (2002) findings of 30%of ‘reversals’ incidence in San’ani Arabic.

According to Carlisle (2001): (5), the reversal patterns areregarded as ‘‘a more serious departure from the sonority

sequencing Principle than sonority plateaus’’. Indeed, 49%

ls of consonant + nasal.

Y. Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-ation (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jksult.2012.12.003

Page 10: Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda …FULL LENGTH ARTICLE Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-based? Yasser A.S. Al Tamimi a,*, Yousef Al Shboul b a

Figure 13 sonority reversals of consonant + voiced fricative.

Figure 14 sonority reversals of consonant + voiceless fricative.

10 Y.A.S. Al Tamimi, Y. Al Shboul

Please cite this article in press as: Al Tamimi, Y.A.S., Al Shboul, Y. Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-based?. Journal of King Saud University – Languages and Translation (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jksult.2012.12.003

Page 11: Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda …FULL LENGTH ARTICLE Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-based? Yasser A.S. Al Tamimi a,*, Yousef Al Shboul b a

Figure 15 sonority reversals of voiceless stop + voiced stop.

Figure 16 Plateau patterns.

Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-based? 11

of sonority reversals for the MSA CC coda casts doubt on theexplanatory adequacy of SSP, traditionally held to account for

strong cross-linguistic distributional and sequential tendencies(Geirut, 1999). Sonority Plateaus to be handled below can pro-vide a further insight.

Figure 17 Identification of sonority plateau patterns.

Please cite this article in press as: Al Tamimi, Y.A.S., Al Shboul, Y. Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-based?. Journal of King Saud University – Languages and Translation (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jksult.2012.12.003

Page 12: Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda …FULL LENGTH ARTICLE Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-based? Yasser A.S. Al Tamimi a,*, Yousef Al Shboul b a

12 Y.A.S. Al Tamimi, Y. Al Shboul

5.3. Sonority plateaus

As shown in Fig. 1 above, sonority plateaus unfold in 44 in-stances out of 493 (9%) that can be categorized into five pat-terns: 1) nasal + nasal (3 instances; 7%), 2) voiced

fricative + voiced fricative (3, 7%), 3) voiceless frica-tive + voiceless fricative (27; 61%), 4) voiced stop + voicedstop (6, 14%), and 5) voiceless stop + voiceless stop (5,11%), as represented in Fig. 16, and identified and exemplified

in Fig. 17.As can be observed in Fig. 16, the pattern of voiceless fric-

ative + voiceless fricative is the largest amongst the sonority

plateau patterns and this can be attributed to the existenceof eight voiceless fricative consonants in MSA phonemicinventory, in contrast with less members in all other natural

classes, as can be seen in Table 1 above.The foregoing provides an exhaustive answer to the third

research question related to sonority plateaus in MSA CC

coda. The occurrence of only 9% of plateaus in this coda isconsistent with Carlisle (2001) generalization that in languages,sonority plateaus are less frequent and marked. It also lendssome support to Watson (2002) findings of some incidence of

sonority plateaus in San’ani Arabic, and, though indirectly,to Gishri (2009) similar findings for Modern Hebrew.

6. Conclusions

Based on an exhaustive quantitative evidence, the study pro-vides a thorough account of the different MSA CC coda pat-

terns and subpatterns that either show conformity to theSonority Sequencing Principle or violate it in the manner ofsonority reversals and plateaus. Contrary to what is taken

for granted in the literature that MSA CC coda normally com-plies with SSP, the study reveals compliance in only 42% of thedata, and, consequently, breaking in 58%; distributed betweensonority reversals (49%) and sonority plateaus (9%).

Although sonority plateaus are ‘‘less dangerous’’ thansonority reversals according to Carlisle (2001): (5), they stillform a violation to the principle. The MSA CC coda is thus

almost divided between compliance and insurgence, with someinclination toward the latter status. This state of affairs poseschallenge to SSP which has been assumed for long to govern

complex onsets and codas in syllables. Accordingly, reconsid-ering a more relevant theoretical model outside the scope ofthe sonority theory is called for. Steriade (1995) Perceptibility

Theory already advocated by Ohala and Kawsaki (1997),Wright (2004), Moreton et al. (2008) might be thought of asa more relevant alternative. The theory simply suggests thata segment’s compatibility within a given environment depends

on how accurately it is likely to be perceived in that environ-ment. Our ongoing research on the acquisition of MSA finalconsonant clusters by Jordanian children indicates that the

perceptive dimension remains a valid option. Further researchis recommended, though, to examine the relevance of the per-ceptibility theory to the phonotactics of MSA complex coda.

References

Al Jumah, A., 2008. The syllable shape of Al-Ahsa dialect: an OT

perspective. Poznan Studies in Contemporary Linguistics 44 (2),

155–177.

Please cite this article in press as: Al Tamimi, Y.A.S., Al Shboul,based?. Journal of King Saud University – Languages and Trans

Al Otaibi, Y., Hussain, A., 2010. Comparative analysis of Arabic

vowels using formants and an automatic speech recognition system.

International Journal of Signal Processing, Image Processing and

Pattern Recognition 3 (2), 11–22.

Al Soswah, A., 2002. al’arabiyyah alfos’a almo’asira wa ‘osooloha

altorathiyyah. Dar Ghareeb, Cairo.

Amayreh, M., 2003. Completion of the consonant inventory of Arabic.

Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 46, 517–529.

Btoosh, M., 2006. Constraint interactions in Jordanian Arabic

phonotactics: an optimality-theoretic approach. Journal of Lan-

guage and Linguistics 5 (2), 192–221.

Carlisle, R., 2001. Syllable structure universals and second language

acquisition. International Journal of English Studies 1, 1–19.

Clements, G.N., 1990. The role of the sonority cycle in syllabification.

In: Kningston, John, Beckman, Mary (Eds.), Papers in Laboratory

Phonology I: Between the Grammar and the Physics of Speech.

Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 283–333.

Daana, H., 2009. The development of consonant clusters, stress and

plural nouns in Jordanian Arabic child language. PhD Disserta-

tion, University of Essex.

Geirut, J., 1999. Syllable onsets: clusters and adjuncts in acquisition.

Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 44, 886–904.

Gishri, M., 2009. The role of prominence in the acquisition of medial

codas in Hebrew. MA Dissertation, Tel Aviv University.

Goldsmith, J., 1990. Autosegmental and Metrical Phonology. Black-

well, Oxford.

Haddad, G., 1983. Problems and issues in the phonology of Lebanese

Arabic. PhD Dissertation. University of Illinois, Urbana-

Champaign.

Hans Wehr, Cowan, J., 1994. The Hans Wehr Dictionary of Modern

Written Arabic (4th ed.). Spoken Language Systems.

Hogg, R., McCully, C., 1987. Metrical Phonology: A Course Book.

Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

Holes, C., 2004. Modern Arabic: Structure, Function and Varieties.

Georgetown University Press.

Jongstra, W., 2003. Variation in reduction strategies of Dutch word-

initial consonant clusters. PhD Dissertation, University of

Toronto.

Kambusziya, A., Serish, M., 2006. Sonority hierarchy principles in

CVCC syllables of Persian. The International Journal of Human-

ities of the Islamic Republic of Iran 13 (1), 107–122.

Kenstowics, M., 1986. Notes on syllable structure in three Arabic

dialects. Journal Revue quebecpise de linguistique 16 (1), 101–

127.

Kiparsky, P., 1979. Metrical structure assignment is cyclic. Linguistic

Inquiry 10, 421–441.

Kiparsky, P., 2003. Syllables and Moras in Arabic. In: Fery, C., Vijver,

R. (Eds.), The Syllable in Optimality Theory. Cambridge Univer-

sity Press, pp. 147–182.

Labov, W., 1966. The Social Stratification of English in York City.

Washington Center for Applied Linguistics.

Ladefoged, P., 1993. A Course in Phonetics. Harcourt Brace, New

York.

Mansour, J., 1991. The Jewish Baghdadi Dialect. The Babylonian

Jewry Heritage Center, Or-Yehuda.

Morellie, F., 1999. The phonotactics and phonology of obstruent

clusters in optimality theory. PhD Dissertation, University of

Maryland.

Moreton, E., Feng, G., Smith, J., 2008. Syllabification, sonority and

perception: new evidence from language game. Proceedings of CLS

41 (1), 341–355.

Obeidat, M., 2010. Consonant ‘h’ and its role in correcting Arabic

syllable structures. Islamic University Journal 18 (2), 825–844.

Ohala, J., Kawsaki, H., 1997. Alternatives to the sonority hierarchy for

explaining segmental constraints. In: Elisson, S., Jahr, E. (Eds.),

Language and its Ecology. Mouton, Berlin, pp. 343–365.

Orzechowska, P., Wiese, R., 2011. Reconstructing the sonority

hierarchy. In: Wai-Sum, Lee, Eric, Zee (Eds.), Proceedings of the

Y. Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-lation (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jksult.2012.12.003

Page 13: Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda …FULL LENGTH ARTICLE Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-based? Yasser A.S. Al Tamimi a,*, Yousef Al Shboul b a

Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-based? 13

Seventh International Conference of Phonetic Science. Hong Kong:

City University of Hong kong. 1542–1547.

Parker, S.G., 2002. Quantifying the sonority hierarchy. PhD Disser-

tation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.

Sievers, E., 1881. Grundzug der Phonetik. Breitkopf and Hartel,

Leipzig.

Steriade, D.,1995. Positional Neutralization. MS, Department of

Linguistics, UCL.

Please cite this article in press as: Al Tamimi, Y.A.S., Al Shboul,based?. Journal of King Saud University – Languages and Transl

Watson, J., 2002. The Phonology and Morphology of Arabic. Oxford

University Press, New York.

Wright, R., 2004. A review of perceptual cues and cue robustness. In:

Hayes, Bruce, Steriade, Donca, Kirchner, Robert (Eds.), Phonet-

ically Based Phonology. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge,

pp. 34–57.

Y. Is the phonotactics of the Arabic complex coda sonority-ation (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jksult.2012.12.003


Recommended