+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Memory and learning

Memory and learning

Date post: 16-Jan-2016
Category:
Upload: totie
View: 32 times
Download: 1 times
Share this document with a friend
Description:
Memory and learning. LeDoux, chapters 5 and 6. Types of memory. There is general agreement that there are several different types of memory, each of which is predominantly in a different part of the brain. Declarative vs. procedural memory. Declarative memory (explicit memory): facts dates - PowerPoint PPT Presentation
Popular Tags:
61
Memory and learning LeDoux, chapters 5 and 6
Transcript
Page 1: Memory and learning

Memory and learning

LeDoux, chapters 5 and 6

Page 2: Memory and learning

Types of memory

• There is general agreement that there are several different types of memory, each of which is predominantly in a different part of the brain.

Page 3: Memory and learning

Declarative vs. procedural memory

• Declarative memory (explicit memory):– facts– dates– events– Hippocampus is critical

• Procedural memory (non-declarative/implicit): – how to perform an act (ride a bicycle)– basal ganglia (dorsal striatum / caudate-putamen) is

critical

Page 4: Memory and learning
Page 5: Memory and learning

• Patients with Alzheimer's disease are unable to learn or remember ordinary facts (declarative memory) but are normal or nearly normal at learning and remembering how to do things (procedural memory).

Page 6: Memory and learning

Memory experiment

• Alzheimer's patients learned and remembered how to read complex words in a mirror as well as normal control subjects

• Were unable to recall the training session or the fact that they had acquired this skill.

Page 7: Memory and learning

Classical (Pavlovian) conditioning

• Another kind of memory is distinct, both behaviorally and anatomically, from declarative or procedural memory.

• Pavlovian conditioning is a form of learning based on the tendency of certain natural events (food presentation) to elicit involuntary responses (salivation) with little or no training.

Page 8: Memory and learning

• Initiating event: Unconditioned stimulus (US)

• Response pattern: Unconditioned response (UR)

• Another "neutral" stimulus (ringing a bell), besides the US, does not usually elicit the UR.

Page 9: Memory and learning

• If another "neutral" stimulus (ringing a bell) is presented simultaneously several times with the Unconditioned stimulus (food), the "neutral" will be able to elicit the Unconditioned response (salivation).

• The "neutral" stimulus (ringing a bell) is then called the Conditioned stimulus (CS).

• The response (salivation) is then called the Conditioned response (CR)

Page 10: Memory and learning

• This process of learning an association between a CS and a CR is called Pavlovian or classical conditioning, or sometimes "associative learning".

• Pavlovian conditioning occurs automatically, with no control, voluntary participation, or (usually) even awareness on the part of the individual to whom it occurs.

Page 11: Memory and learning

• Evidence in animals and humans indicate that the amygdala is critical for classical conditioning.

• The studies indicate that the amygdala mediates expression of conditioned rewarding and approach behaviors as well as conditioned aversive and escape responses (e.g., "freezing" in mice).

Page 12: Memory and learning

Fear conditioning

• A simple form of associative learning (Pavlovian conditioning)

• Animals learn to "fear" a previously neutral stimulus (conditioned stimulus, CS), because the US has been presented at the same time as an aversive stimulus (unconditioned stimulus, US) such as a foot shock.

• Conditioned animals, when exposed to the CS, tend to refrain from all movement except breathing ("freezing").

Page 13: Memory and learning

• Freezing responses can be triggered with two different types of CS, each working via different parts of the brain:

• - In "cued conditioning", the CS is simply a tone (e.g., 85 dB, 2800 Hz), and lesions in the amygdala, but not the hippocampus, appear to disrupt this type of conditioning.

• - In "contextual conditioning", rodents become conditioned to the "context" in which they were exposed, such as a particular location. Contextual conditioning is thought to depend on both the amygdala and the hippocampus.

Page 14: Memory and learning

Memory and the hippocampus

• In 1950, a young man, known now by his initials, H.M. underwent brain surgery in Hartford, Connecticut.

• H.M. was one of several patients in whom parts of the temporal lobe were removed in an effort to control epilepsy.

Page 15: Memory and learning

• The temporal lobe is one of the four major divisions (lobes) of the brain, and is often the place in the brain attacked by epilepsy.

• In H.M’s case, temporal lobe areas were removed on both sides of his brain.

• After the surgery, his epilepsy was better, but he no longer had the ability to acquire new memories.

Page 16: Memory and learning

• H.M became probably the most famous case in neurological history, and has been the subject of many studies.

• Much of the initial work was carried out by Brenda Milner and her colleagues in Montreal.

Page 17: Memory and learning

• Milner found that, although H.M could recall many of the events of his earlier life, he was unable to form new memories for experiences that occurred after the surgery.

• He could remember things for a few seconds (short-term memories) but he couldn’t convert this information into long-term memories.

Page 18: Memory and learning

• Analysis of H.M.’s lesion, based on the surgical report, indicated that the main temporal lobe areas affected were the hippocampus, amygdala, and parts of the surrounding cortex.

• By comparing H.M.’s lesion with those in other patients, it seemed that the hippocampus was the area damaged most consistently in memory deficits.

Page 19: Memory and learning

• At first, it was thought that H.M. had lost all ability to acquire new memories.

• However, it was found that he could learn certain tasks.

Page 20: Memory and learning

• Brenda Milner asked H.M. to copy a picture of a star viewed through a mirror.

• To do this, the movements had to be done in the direction opposite from the way the seemed they should be made.

• This task is hard at first, but eventually most people can do it, and H.M. was no exception.

Page 21: Memory and learning

• H.M. learning the mirror drawing task, and he retained the learning.

• But if asked about drawing using the mirror, he had no conscious memory of having done it.

Page 22: Memory and learning

• Suzanne Corkin of MIT found that H.M. also improved with practice in another manual skill learning task – one in which he was required to keep a stick held in his hand on a dot spinning on a turntable.

• As with the mirror drawing task, the more times he did it, the better he got.

• His ability to form memories about how to make precise movements (motor skills) seemed intact.

• Subsequent work has shown that other regions of the brain (the basal ganglia) are primarily responsible for remembering motor skills.

Page 23: Memory and learning

• Much is now known about the hippocampus, but we will mention just a few points.

• Information about the external world comes into the brain through sensory systems that relay signals to the cortex, where sensory representations of objects and events are created

Page 24: Memory and learning

• Outputs of each of the cortical sensory systems converge in parahippocampal region (also known as the rhinal cortical areas) which surrounds the hippocampus.

• The parahippocampal region integrates information from the different sensory modalities before sending it to the hippocampus proper.

Page 25: Memory and learning

• The hippocampus and parahippocampal region make up what is now called the medial temporal lobe memory system, which is involved in explicit or declarative memory.

Page 26: Memory and learning
Page 27: Memory and learning

• The connections between the hippocampus and the neocortex are all more or less reciprocal

• The pathways that take information from the neocortex to the rhinal areas and then into the hippocampus are mirrored by pathways going in the opposite direction.

• Cortical areas involved in processing a stimulus can thereby also participate in the long-term storage of memories of that stimulus.

Page 28: Memory and learning

• The rhinal areas serves as convergence zones, brain regions that integrate information across sensory modalities and create representations that are independent of the original modailty.

• As a result, sights, sounds, and smells can be put together in the form of a global memory of a situation.

Page 29: Memory and learning
Page 30: Memory and learning

• Convergence zones allow mental representations to go beyond perceptions to become conceptions.

• They make possible abstract representations that are independent of concrete stimulus.

• The primate neocortex has several cortical areas, more than in other mammals.

Page 31: Memory and learning

• Because the hippocampus receives inputs from several convergence zones in the rhinal region, it can be thought of as a superconvergence zone.

• It can form explicit memories about many domain-specific systems, such as face- and language-processing systems, allowing us, for example, to form a memory that includes both what someone says and what he looks like.

Page 32: Memory and learning

• Many researchers believe that explicit memories are stored in the corical systems that were involved in the initial processing of the stimulus, and that the hippocampus is needed to direct the storage process.

Page 33: Memory and learning

Early experiments on drug effects on memory

• Certain post-training treatments can modulate memory storage in ways that enhance or prevent retention.

• First observed with stimulant drugs– strychnine (very low doses)– amphetamine– caffeine

Page 34: Memory and learning

• Early studies showed that drugs that inhibit protein synthesis also inhibit long-term memory formation.

• Several inhibitors of RNA synthesis or protein synthesis block long-term memory, but do not affect short-term memory.

Page 35: Memory and learning

Gene transcription, translation, and memory

• DNA is transcribed to produce RNA• RNA is translated to produce protein• DNA -> RNA -> protein

• Transcription factors are proteins that regulate what genes are transcribed (expressed).

• Transcription factors typically bind near the promoter region of a gene (the on/off switch).

Page 36: Memory and learning

Amphetamine improves learning

• Rats were put into a cage where they could drink water.

• After being put in the cage, the rats heard a series of 10 second tones, each terminated with a brief foot shock.

• The shock caused the animals to stop moving (freeze).

• After several such tone-shock pairings, the rats acquired a conditioned freezing response, which lasted for several minutes each time the tone was presented.

Page 37: Memory and learning

• The next day, the rats were placed in the drinking cage.

• Tone came on when they began to drink.• Animals froze• Duration of freezing was used as a measure of

the rats' memory for the tone-freezing association.

• Rats that experiences more pairings (12) froze significantly longer than rats that had fewer pairings (2).

Page 38: Memory and learning

• Some rats got drug injections immediately after their experience of the tone-shock pairings.

• Rats that got two pairings followed immediately by a saline injection froze for slightly longer than rats that got two pairings but no injection.

• However, rats that got two pairings followed immediately by an amphetamine injection froze for about the same length of time as rats that got 12 pairings (but no drug).

• => Amphetamine improves learning if it is given immediately after training

Page 39: Memory and learning

• Another group of rats got 2 pairings followed by amphetamine injection 2 hours later.

• These rats froze for the same length of time as rates that received saline or no injection,

• => amphetamine had no effect if it was given 2 hours after the training

Page 40: Memory and learning

• The results indicate that the immediate amphetamine injections improved the rats' memory for the tone-freezing association.

• The fact that the delayed drug injection had no effect is consistent with the idea that the memory was susceptible to modulation only during a consolidation period that lasted less than two hours.

Page 41: Memory and learning

How drugs act on synapses

• Neurons communicate with each other at synapses using chemical neurotransmitters.

• This provides the bases for drugs (and poisons) to affect synaptic transmission.

• Drugs with chemical properties similar in some way to those of neurotransmitters can act on synapses to alter behavior and thoughts (psychotropic or psychoactive drugs)

Page 42: Memory and learning

• Drugs that increase synaptic transmission are "agonists".

• Drugs that block or reduce synaptic transmission are "antagonists".

Page 43: Memory and learning
Page 44: Memory and learning

• About 25 neurotransmitters are known in the mammalian brain.

• Most psychoactive drugs act on the synapses of a single neurotransmitter.

• These synapses often occur in different, functionally unrelated parts of the brain, controlling many different behaviors

• The psychological actions of drugs can be quite complex and difficult to predict

Page 45: Memory and learning

To affect the brain, drugs must cross the blood-brain barrier

• Access to the brain from the circulatory system is controlled by the blood-brain barrier (BBB).

• This barrier is made up of a layer of cell surrounding the blood vessels that supply the brain.

• These cells determine the degree to which substances in the blood can enter the brain.

Page 46: Memory and learning

• Fat-soluble substances (e.g., alcohol) cross the BBB more easily than water –soluble substances.

• Drugs and hormones with large molecular weights do not easily pass the BBB.

• Some substances, including glucose and insulin, are actively transported into the brain.

• The degree to which drugs cross the BBB is critical to their effects on memory.

Page 47: Memory and learning

Learning and memory

• What is the nature of the neural changes that constitute learning and memory?

• Most neuroscientists today believe that alterations in synaptic connectivity underlie learning, and that memory is the stabilization and maintenance of these changes over time.

• How does experience change synapses, and what makes changes last?

Page 48: Memory and learning

• Changes in synapses resulting from the simultaneous (or near simultaneous) activation of neurons is generally thought to be the basis of all learning, including procedural, declarative, and conditioned learning.

• We will see that the central role of synaptic changes in learning and memory provides the bases for the action of neurologic drugs.

Page 49: Memory and learning

• By the early 1950’s, several studies had shown that repeated delivery of a brief electrical stimulus to a nerve pathway could alter synaptic transmission in that pathway – could, in other words, produce synaptic plasticity.

Page 50: Memory and learning

Hebbian learning

• The most-widely accepted theory of how information is stored in the nervous system is based on a concept first described by D.O. Hebb, now called Hebbian learning.

• Start with the idea that each perception evokes a unique set pattern of neural activity.

• The set of activated neurons are connected to each other, and reactivate each other for a short period of time.

Page 51: Memory and learning

• Hebb suggested that this period of recurrent activation repeatedly activates the synapses connecting the neurons, causing the synapses to undergo permanent changes. These changes facilitate future activation of the synapses.

• The pattern of permanently facilitated synapses increases the probability that on future occasions activation of one part of some of the neurons will activate the rest of the neurons, leading to recall of the information it represents.

Page 52: Memory and learning

Hebb’s proposal

• “when an axon of cell A is near enough to excite cell B or repeatedly and consistently takes part in firing it, some growth process or metabolic changes take place in one or both cells such that A’s efficiency, as one of the cells firing B, is increased.”

Page 53: Memory and learning
Page 54: Memory and learning

Memory consolidation

• When first acquired, memories are stored in a labile state (represented by Hebb's recurrent activation phase) and are subject to disruption by external events.

• With the passage of time their storage may become more permanent (Hebb's synaptic changes) and are less susceptible to disruption.

• This process by which memories become permanent is called "consolidation". The interval during which the hypothesized process of synaptic change occurs is called the consolidation period.

Page 55: Memory and learning

Long Term Potentiation: A mechanism for memory

• In the mid-1960’s Terje Lomo in Oslo noticed that a brief burst of electrical stimuli delivered to nerves going to the hippocampus in a rabbit led to a dramatic and long-lasting increase in transmission (a bigger electrical response to a test stimulus after, as compared to before, the burst) at synapses in the hippocampus.

• This is now called Long Term Potentiation (LTP)

Page 56: Memory and learning

• Later, Lomo and Bliss put a stimulating electrode in the nerve pathway going into the hippocampus and a recording electrode in the hippocampus itself.

• They delivered a single electrical stimulus to the pathway, and recorded the electrical response of the postsynaptic neurons.

• This served as the baseline, the standard against which the rest of the experiment was gauged.

Page 57: Memory and learning

• Next, they gave the potentiating stimulus – a brief burst of many repeated pulses. Then they started testing again with a single pulse, and continued testing periodically for several hours.

• They found that, after the potentiating pulses, the synaptic response got bigger, relative to the baseline response, and remained bigger for hours.

Page 58: Memory and learning
Page 59: Memory and learning

• Although Bliss and Lomo had produced long-lasting change in the postsynaptic response by electrically stimulating neural pathways, rather than by having the animals actually learn something, they realized that they had identified a mechanism that might be able to translate neural activity generated by environmental stimuli into changes in synaptic efficiency.

Page 60: Memory and learning

• This mechanism might be used for learning and memory.

• The fact that the discovery was made in hippocampal tissue supported their speculation that changes in the efficiency of synaptic transmission might account for memory.

• LTP has since been induced in many areas of the nervous system.

Page 61: Memory and learning

• Computer simulations have demonstrated that information (memories) can be stored and recalled in a "neural network" in which the weights between "neurons" are altered as a result of learning.


Recommended