+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Musical Obsessions

Musical Obsessions

Date post: 07-Aug-2018
Category:
Upload: vinodksahu
View: 219 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend

of 24

Transcript
  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    1/61

    Accepted Manuscript

    Title: Musical obsessions: A comprehensive review of 

    neglected clinical phenomena

    Author: Steven Taylor Dean McKay Euripedes C. Miguel

    Maria Alice De Mathis Chittaranjan Andrade Niraj AhujaDebbie Sookman Jun Soo Kwon Min Jung Huh Bradley C.

    Riemann Jean Cottraux Kieron O’Connor Lisa R. Hale

    Jonathan S. Abramowitz Leonardo F. Fontenelle Eric A.

    Storch

    PII: S0887-6185(14)00085-1

    DOI:   http://dx.doi.org/doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2014.06.003

    Reference: ANXDIS 1613

    To appear in:   Journal of Anxiety Disorders

    Received date: 14-11-2013

    Revised date: 9-5-2014

    Accepted date: 6-6-2014

    Please cite this article as: Taylor, S., McKay, D., Miguel, E. C., Mathis, M. A. D.,Andrade, C., Ahuja, N., Sookman, D., Kwon, J. S., Huh, M. J., Riemann, B. C.,

    Cottraux, J., O’Connor, K., Hale, L. R., Abramowitz, J. S., Fontenelle, L. F., and Storch,

    E A M sical obsessions: A comprehensi e re ie of neglected clinical phenomena

    http://dx.doi.org/doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2014.06.003http://dx.doi.org/doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2014.06.003

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    2/61

    E A Musical obsessions: A comprehensive review of neglected clinical phenomena

    pted

        M  a  n   u  s  c  r   i

       p   t

     Highlights: 

     

    This is the first comprehensive review of musical obsessions.

      It was based on the largest sample of published and unpublished cases ever

    assembled.

      Phenomenology, differential diagnosis, comorbidity, and treatments are reviewed.

     

    Musical obsessions differ from other obsessions in phenomenology and treatment.

      Etiological hypotheses and important directions for future research are presented.

    *Highlights (for review)

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    3/61

    pted

        M  a  n   u  s  c  r   i

       p   t

    1

    Running head: Musical Obsessions

    Musical Obsessions: A Comprehensive Review of Neglected Clinical Phenomena

    Steven Taylora*, Dean McKay

     b, Euripedes C. Miguel

    c, Maria Alice De Mathis

    c,

    Chittaranjan Andrade d, Niraj Ahuja e, Debbie Sookman f , Jun Soo Kwon g, Min Jung

    Huhg, Bradley C. Riemann

    h, Jean Cottraux

    i, Kieron O‘Connor

     j, Lisa R. Hale

    k ,

    Jonathan S. Abramowitzl, Leonardo F. Fontenelle

    m, & Eric A. Storch

    h, n 

    aUniversity of British Columbia, BC, Canada

     b

    Fordham University, NY, USAcUniversity of São Paulo Medical School, Brazil

    d National Institute of Mental Health and Neurosciences, India

    e Northumberland, Tyne and Wear NHS Foundation Trust, UK

    fMcGill University, PQ, Canada

    g

    Seoul National University, Republic of Koreah

    Rogers Memorial Hospital, WI, USA

    iUniversity Lyon 1, France

    *Manuscript

    Click here to view linked References

    http://ees.elsevier.com/anxdis/viewRCResults.aspx?pdf=1&docID=3168&rev=1&fileID=50228&msid={08B6C796-D1CD-461C-AB89-5AE8076E0C6B}http://ees.elsevier.com/anxdis/viewRCResults.aspx?pdf=1&docID=3168&rev=1&fileID=50228&msid={08B6C796-D1CD-461C-AB89-5AE8076E0C6B}

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    4/61

    pted

        M  a  n   u  s  c  r   i

       p   t

    2

    Abstract

    Intrusive musical imagery (IMI) consists of involuntarily recalled, short, looping

    fragments of melodies. Musical obsessions are distressing, impairing forms of IMI that

    merit investigation in their own right and, more generally, research into these phenomena

    may broaden our understanding of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), which is

     phenomenologically and etiologically heterogeneous. We present the first comprehensive

    review of musical obsessions, based on the largest set of case descriptions ever assembled

    ( N  = 96). Characteristics of musical obsessions are described and compared with normal

    IMI, musical hallucinations, and visual obsessional imagery. Assessment, differential

    diagnosis, comorbidity, etiologic hypotheses, and treatments are described. Musical

    obsessions may be under-diagnosed because they are not adequately assessed by current

    measures of OCD. Musical obsessions have been misdiagnosed as psychotic phenomena,

    which has led to ineffective treatment. Accurate diagnosis is important for appropriate

    treatment. Musical obsessions may respond to treatments that are not recommended for

     prototypic OCD symptoms.

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    5/61

    pted

        M  a  n   u  s  c  r   i

       p   t

    3

     Keywords: Obsessive-compulsive disorder, intrusive musical imagery, involuntary

    musical imagery, earworms, orhwurms, musical obsessions.

     Highlights: 

      This is the first comprehensive review of musical obsessions.

     

    It was based on the largest sample of published and unpublished cases ever

    assembled.

      Phenomenology, differential diagnosis, comorbidity, and treatments are reviewed.

      Musical obsessions differ in several ways from other obsessions regarding

     phenomenology and treatment.

      Etiological hypotheses and important directions for future research are presented.

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    6/61

    pted

        M  a  n   u  s  c  r   i

       p   t

    4

    1. Introduction

    Obsessions and compulsions are the cardinal features of obsessive-compulsive

    disorder (OCD). Obsessions are unwanted, intrusive, and distressing thoughts, images, or

    urges. Compulsions are repetitive behaviors or cognitive rituals that the person feels

    compelled to perform, often in response to obsessions (American Psychiatric Association

    [APA], 2013). Although intrusive mental imagery has long been recognized as a salient

    feature of OCD (de Silva, 1986), clinical descriptions and research have focused almost

    exclusively on visual imagery. Phenomena involving intrusive musical imagery (IMI),

    consisting of intrusive recollections of fragments of music (i.e., music running through

    one‘s mind), have been neglected by contemporary OCD researchers. This is surprising

     because descriptions of IMI appeared in the psychological and psychiatric literature for

    over a century (e.g., Ebbinghaus, 1885; Janet, 1903; Kraepelin, 1915), and IMI continues

    to be studied by contemporary musicologists and neurologists (Liikkanen, 2012b; Sacks,

    2007). IMI is almost universally experienced (Liikkanen, 2012b). Underscoring the

    importance of IMI, the neurologist Oliver Sacks (2007) observed that it is ―the clearest

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    7/61pted

        M  a  n   u  s  c  r   i

       p   t

    5

    such as washing or checking compulsions. A comprehensive understanding requires that

    even the less common symptoms be investigated in order to gain a better understanding

    of OCD.

    Although there have been few studies of musical obsessions compared to studies

    of other OC phenomena, this is not necessarily an indication of the clinical insignificance

    of musical obsessions. Historically, clinical investigators have tended to underestimate

    the prevalence and importance of OC-related phenomena. OCD was once considered

    rare. It was not until the epidemiologic surveys were conducted that OCD came to be

    recognized as a ―hidden epidemic‖ (Jenike, 1989). Similarly, the prevalence and

    significance of excessive hoarding — an OC-related phenomenon — was not fully

    appreciated until recently (Mueller, Mitchell, Crosby, Glaesmer, & de Zwaan, 2009).

    Even as late as 1987, descriptive case studies of hoarding were sufficiently rare as to

    merit publication (Greenberg, 1987).

    To our knowledge, the present paper is the first comprehensive review of musical

    obsessions, based on the largest ever assembled set of case descriptions ( N = 96,

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    8/61pted

        M  a  n   u  s  c  r   i

       p   t

    6

    2. Intrusive Musical Imagery

    IMI is characterized by a tune that repeatedly comes to mind without the person

    consciously trying to recall it (Williamson et al., 2011). Also known as earworms,

    ohrwurms, or involuntary musical imagery, IMI is a form of intrusive, auditory imagery

    occurring in the absence of neurological pathology or ear disease. In a survey of over

    5,000 participants, Stafford (2012) found that IMI usually —  but not invariably — consists

    of simple repetitive song fragments, such as a particular verse or ―hook‖ that has an

    ineffable ―catchy‖ quality. IMI need not be limited to simple or rhythmically repetitive

    songs; almost any song can be a source of IMI (Hyman et al., 2013). IMI involves a

    fragment of music that the person has previously heard, such as a chorus or line of music,

    equal to or less than the capacity of auditory short-term memory. That is, repetitions of

    15-30 second segments of music, persisting like a looping soundtrack (Bailes, 2007;

    Beaman & Williams, 2010; Liikkanen, 2012b,c). Repetitions of IMI may last from

    minutes to hours (Halpern & Bartlett, 2011), but most often consist of recurrent,

    intermittent episodes rather than a continuous musical soundtrack (Hyman et al., 2013).

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    9/61pted

        M  a  n   u  s  c  r   i

       p   t

    7

    to people or places associated with a particular piece of music (Halpern & Bartlett, 2011;

    Hyman et al., 2013; Liikkanen, 2012a; Williamson et al., 2011). IMI can also be triggered

     by personally important events (Janata, 2009; Reik, 1953). For example, music played at

    a funeral might become a source of later IMI.

    Surveys indicate that more than 85% of people in the general population

    experience involuntary musical imagery at least weekly (Bailes, 2007; Bennett, 2002;

    Liikkanen, 2012b). The frequency of IMI is positively correlated with the amount of

    music involvement (e.g., amount of practice or listening to music), and the perceived

    importance of music (Beaman & Williams, 2010; Bennett, 2002; Floridou, Williamson,

    & Müllensiefen, 2012; Liikkanen, 2012b).

    For most people, IMI is not aversive (Beaman & Williams, 2010; Halpern &

    Bartlett, 2011; Hyman et al., 2013). In fact, some songs and advertising jingles become

     popular precisely because they induce IMI. People who find IMI to be aversive are more

    likely to have prototypic OC symptoms, such as washing or checking rituals. To

    illustrate, Williamson and Müllensiefen (2012) administered measures of IMI and OC

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    10/61pted

        M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    8

    were caused by a third variable, such as a vulnerability factor for OC symptoms (see

     below for etiologic considerations). Nevertheless, the findings show that prototypic OC

    symptoms and IMI-related distress are correlated with one another.

    3. Musical Obsessions: Descriptive Psychopathology

    3.1. Diagnostic Criteria

    Musical obsessions are not mentioned in the most recent version of the Diagnostic

    and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5; APA, 2013). Musical obsessions are

    described in the clinical literature as episodes of IMI that meet criteria for OC symptoms;

    that is, the obsessional imagery is recurrent, persistent, intrusive, unintentional, time

    consuming (i.e., more than an hour per day), and causes distress or functional

    impairment. Note that this definition refers to persistent, involuntary musical imagery. It

    does not refer to obsessional preoccupations about particular pieces of music. A woman

    with an obsessional fear of becoming a lesbian, for example, might fear and avoid songs

    with homoerotic lyrics, such as Katy Perry‘s I Kissed a Girl and I Liked it. This is not a

    musical obsession as the term is used. Similarly, the definition of musical obsessions

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    11/61pted

        M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    9

    3.2. Differential Diagnosis

    3.2.1. Musical hallucinations. Hallucinations are perceptions lacking an adequate

    input stimulus, which are perceived to arise from an external source and are interpreted as

    veridical (Sedman, 1966). Musical hallucinations are rare (Berrios, 1990). They have

     been described in patients with acquired hearing loss, epilepsy, structural brain lesions,

     psychosis, delirium, drug induced states, and hypnagogic states (Evers & Ellger, 2004).

    In contrast to musical hallucinations, musical obsessions are a form of imagery that the

     person recognizes as originating from his or her mind, and occur in people who do not

    suffer from hearing disorders or neurological pathology.

    3.2.2. Pseudohallucinations. The concept of pseudohallucinations is problematic

     because of the lack of consensus regarding its definition and distinction from true

    hallucinations (Sanati, 2012). However, it is generally agreed that pseudohallucinations

    are forms of imagery that occur in inner (subjective) space and are recognized by the

     person as not veridical perceptions. To this extent, musical pseudohallucinations

    resemble musical obsessions. The two differ in that pseudohallucinations need not be

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    12/61pted

        M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    10

    obsessions; that is, the person always recognizes that the musical imagery is a product of

    his or her mind. In comparison, insight can fluctuate in musical pseudohallucinations; at

    times the person might believe that the source of the music is internal, but at times may

     believe that it comes from an external source (Sanati, 2012).

    3.2.3. Palinacousis. This is a neurological symptom consisting of episodes of

     paroxysmal auditory hallucination, in which perseveration (echoing) of an external

    auditory stimulus occurs after cessation of the stimulus. The person recognizes the

    hallucinatory nature of the experience, which is often described as ―hearing‖ a fragment

    of music, a noise, or portions of speech (Jacobs, Feldman, Diamond, & Bender, 1973;

    Mohamed, Ahuja, & Shah, 2012). Palinacousis is rare and musical palinacousis is even

    rarer (Mohamed et al., 2012; Podoll, 2010). Palinacousis is associated with EEG and

    neuroimaging abnormalities, with structural brain pathology (e.g., cortical atrophy,

     primary tumors, and metastases), typically in the temporal or parietal lobes (Mohamed et

    al., 2012). Palinacousis is usually a manifestation of seizure activity (e.g., an aura),

    although it may also be a post-ictal or a migrainous phenomenon (Di Dio et al., 2007;

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    13/61pted

        M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    11

    3.3.1. Acquisition of published case material. A literature search of the following

    databases was conducted up to May 1, 2014: PsychInfo, MEDLINE, EMBASE, and

    Google Scholar. The keywords were obsess* and music* (the asterisks denote the use of

    wild cards). Articles in all languages were considered. References in the identified

    articles were examined for additional relevant material. Researchers who had previously

     published on musical obsessions were also contacted for published and unpublished

    cases. A total of 17 publications were identified (Ahuja, 2001; Akhtar, Wig, Varma,

    Pershad, & Verma, 1975; Andrade & Rao, 1997; Berg, 1953; Cameron & Wasielewski,

    1990; Fontenelle, 2008; Gomibuchi, Gomibuchi, Akiyama, Tsuda, & Hayakawa, 2000;

    Hemming & Altenmüller, 2012; Maršanić, Aukst-Margetić, Grgić, & Kušmić, 2011;

    Matsui et al., 2003; Mendhekar & Andrade, 2009; Nath, Bhattacharya, Hazarika, Roy, &

    Praharaj, 2013; Pfizer & Andrade, 1999; Praharaj et al., 2009; Rapoport, 1980; Saha,

    2012; Zungu-Dirwayi et al., 1999). The five cases reported by Hemming and Altenmüller

    (2012) were excluded because most suffered from tinnitus.

    Berg (1953) summarized findings from 10 cases, Gomibuchi et al. (2000)

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    14/61pted

        M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    12

    3.3.2. Additional cases. To supplement the published case studies, the authors of

    the present paper  — clinicians with expertise in OCD — reviewed their own clinical case

    files for previously unpublished cases of musical obsessions. All cases had been

    evaluated with Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale (Y-BOCS; Goodman et al.,

    1989). The patients had been evaluated in a variety of settings, ranging from private

     practices focusing on the treatment of anxiety and related problems to large-scale hospital

    or research clinics specializing in OCD and related problems. The number of patient files

    that were examined ranged from a few dozen to databases consisting of over a thousand

    clinical records. We obtained information on 63 unpublished cases. Thus, we had

    information on a total of 96 previously published and new cases of musical obsessions.

    To the best of our knowledge, this is by far the largest set of cases of musical obsessions

    ever assembled.

    The representativeness of the published and unpublished cases cannot be

    determined on the basis of the available information; that would require epidemiologic

    research, which has yet to be conducted. Nevertheless the published and new cases

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    15/61pted

        M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    13

    unemployed (4%).

    3.3.3. Diagnostic features. With the exception of Berg (1953), for which

    diagnostic status was unclear, all cases were diagnosed with OCD. There was no

    evidence that any of these individuals suffered from palinacousis (e.g., none had a history

    of seizures) and none appeared to be suffering from tinnitus or deafness. Musical

    obsessions of two patients were initially misdiagnosed as symptoms of schizophrenia.

    However, neither they nor any of the other cases had psychotic features such as formal

    thought disorder or verbal auditory hallucinations. Neurological factors (brain

    inflammation or injury) might have contributed to three cases of musical obsessions,

    discussed below, but even these cases had unremarkable findings on neurological

    evaluations (e.g., EEG, neuroimaging).

    3.3.4. Content of obsessions. Musical obsessions were either the sole presenting

     problem or occurred in the context of other psychopathology. Musical obsessions were

    either primary or secondary to other presenting problems in terms of severity. Musical

    obsessions consisted of either a single song or a series of songs that changed over time, as

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    16/61pted

        M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    14

    intermittently and in other cases were reported as occurring almost continuously during

    waking hours.

    3.3.5. Fear, avoidance, and compulsions related to musical obsessions. It is

    common for people with OCD, regardless of the type of symptoms, to attempt to cope

    with their obsessions by engaging in avoidance behaviors or some form of compensatory

    ritual (Abramowitz et al., 2011). For example, someone with visual obsessional imagery

    of harming their child might avoid kitchen knives, seek reassurance from others about the

    child‘s well-being, and attempt to suppress or eliminate the unwanted violent images. We

    found evidence of analogous behaviors in the cases of musical obsessions.

    In some cases of musical obsessions, patients developed fear and avoidance of

    stimuli that triggered musical obsessions, including avoidance of stimuli or situations

    associated with music, such as avoiding listening to music or avoiding public places

    where music is commonly played. To illustrate, a patient with ring tone obsessions went

    to great lengths to avoid exposure to ring tones. He wore earplugs, kept his phone on

    silent (vibrate) mode, urged others to do the same, and avoided places where he might

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    17/61pted

        M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    15

    ring tone obsessions the patient had enjoyed downloading, previewing, and changing ring

    tones on his cellular phone, devoting up to 2-3 hours/day to this activity. He discontinued

    the activity as he became increasingly troubled by recurrent unwanted intrusive imagery

    of ring tones.

    The onset and exacerbation of musical obsessions in other cases appeared to be

    stress related. In several cases, musical obsessions developed in students who were

    studying for exams. In another case, musical obsessions consisted of funeral dirges,

    which develo ped shortly after the death of the patient‘s wife. In yet another case the

    obsessions began after the patient resigned from his job due to interpersonal conflicts. In

    one case the musical obsession appeared to be the result of an attempt by the patient to

    distract herself from other obsessions. She developed the strategy of singing to herself to

    avoid or neutralize her other, non-musical obsessions, but then began experiencing

    unwanted, distressing musical imagery of the songs she had used as distraction.

    There was no evidence of neurological abnormalities in almost all cases. In three

    cases, however, onset might have been triggered by a neurological insult. In one case

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    18/61pted

        M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    16

    In a second case (Pfizer & Andrade, 1999), musical obsessions arose suddenly, in

    conjunction with an infection causing sore throat and fever. The infection was

    successfully treated with erythromycin but the musical obsessions persisted. The clinical

     presentation resembled the class of pediatric autoimmune neuropsychiatric disorders

    associated with streptococcal infection (PANDAS). PANDAS-related OCD is said to

    arise from infection-related inflammation of the basal ganglia and associated structures.

    The validity of the concept of PANDAS is contentious (Murphy, Kurlan, & Leckman,

    2010); ―Despite more than a decade of studying … PANDAS, it is still not possible to

    confirm its existence and whether it is a post-streptococcal autoimmune disorder‖ 

    (Knupp, de Oliveira, & Pelajo, 2010, p. 103). Pfizer and Andrade (1999) concluded that

    the association between infection and the onset of musical obsessions was probably

    coincidental.

    In a third case (Zungu-Dirwayi et al., 1999, Case 2), musical obsessions

    developed six months after a closed head injury. The patient was unconscious for four

    days and had a month of retrograde amnesia. A CT scan was normal although there was

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    19/61pted

        M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    17

    and aggressive, horrific, sexual, or religious obsessions. Some of the comorbid

    obsessions consisted of distressing visual imagery. Some patients had comorbid

    obsessions that were similar in form to musical obsessions; for example, unwanted,

    intrusive recollections of verses of spoken poetry. Comorbidity of musical obsessions

    with many other types of OC symptoms is consistent with OCD in general, in which such

    comorbidity is common (APA, 2013).

    Several patients were described as having features of obsessive-compulsive

     personality disorder (i.e., extreme perfectionism or preoccupation with orderliness). One

     patient was described as suffering from distressing pareidolias, that is, illusory images

    that were perceived while looking at shapes (e.g., seeing gorillas in the mosaic pattern of

    floor tiles). Another patient exhibited misophonia, which is characterized by intolerance

    and distress in response to minor everyday sounds such as the sound of other people

    eating (Møller, 2011). Disorders that are commonly comorbid with OCD in general were

    also described, such as other anxiety disorders, mood disorders, and tics. There was no

    evidence of psychotic features in the 96 cases. In summary, for most cases the pattern of

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    20/61pted

        M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    18

    Current measures of OC symptoms and OCD fail to adequately assess musical

    obsessions, which makes it difficult to estimate their prevalence. Only one item of the

    symptom checklist of the Y-BOCS assesses whether the respondent has suffered from

    ―intrusive nonsense sounds, words, or music‖, with the example given as ―words, songs,

    or music in your mind that you can‘t stop.‖ This item fails to distinguish musical

    obsessions from other intrusive auditory imagery.

    4. Musical Obsessions Compared to Obsessions

    Characterized by Intrusive Visual Imagery

    To further clarify the nature of musical obsessions, it is useful to consider how

    they are similar to, and different from, other types of obsessional imagery. Most

    obsessional imagery is visual (de Silva, 1986; Speckens et al., 2007). Obsessions

    characterized by spoken words, olfactory, gustatory, or tactile imagery are rare (Chauhan,

    Shah, & Grover, 2010; de Silva, 1986; Speckens et al., 2007). Unwanted visual images

    are typically inherently aversive or violate the person‘s values or beliefs. Common

    examples include images of violently attacking one‘s child or elderly parents,

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    21/61pted

        M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    19

    ego-dystonic material such as unwanted images that violate the person‘s religious, sexual,

    or moral values. The intrusive music is usually not something that the person associates

    with some unpleasant event, although there are some exceptions (e.g., the funeral dirge

    case). Musical and visual obsessional imagery also differ in that the former involves

    music that is typically familiar to the person; that is, something that the person has

    actually heard or composed. In comparison, visual obsessional imagery typically consists

    of imagined scenarios. Musical and visual obsessional imagery also differ to some extent

    in their treatment. Imaginal (and situational) exposure and response prevention is an

    empirically supported treatment for OCD in general, including the treatment of visual

    obsessional imagery (Abramowitz, Deacon, & Whiteside, 2011).These methods have

     been used to treat musical obsessions, but distraction has also been successfully used in

    case studies (see below). Distraction is not an empirically supported treatment for visual

    obsessions, as discussed below. Musical and visual obsessional imagery are similar in

    that both are commonly comorbid with other OC symptoms, such as washing, checking,

    and ordering compulsions. Our case series indicates that it is not uncommon for musical

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    22/61pted

        M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    20

    extended to account for musical obsessions. In the following section we summarize these

    models and discuss how they might be modified to explain why most people experience

    IMI but relatively few develop musical obsessions.

    5. Etiological Considerations

    5.1. Contemporary Cognitive-Behavioral Models of OC Symptoms

    Although there is no definitive etiological model of obsessions and compulsions,

    contemporary cognitive-behavioral models are among the leading approaches (Frost &

    Steketee, 2002; Salkovskis, 1996). These models, which share basic postulates, propose

    that OC symptoms arise from specific types of dysfunctional beliefs, where the content

    and strength of belief influences the development and severity of OC symptoms. Three

    inter-correlated types of beliefs have been theoretically and empirically linked to OC

    symptoms: (a) perfectionism (P) and intolerance of uncertainty (C; collectively referred

    to as PC), (b) over-importance of thoughts and the need to control thoughts (ICT), and (c)

    inflated responsibility and the overestimation of threat (RT) (Obsessive Compulsive

    Cognitions Working Group, 2005). PC involves beliefs that mistakes and imperfection

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    23/61pted

        M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    21

    transforming ordinary intrusions into obsessions. To illustrate, consider the unwanted

    intrusive image of stabbing a loved one. If the person interprets the intrusion as having no

    significance (i.e., regarded it as mental detritus) then he or she would experience little

    distress or impairment due to the unwanted image. In comparison, if a person interpreted

    the intrusion as an indication that he or she is at risk for losing control and inflicting

    harm, then the person would become distressed, attempt to vigorously suppress such

    imagery, avoid triggering stimuli (e.g., kitchen knives), and repeatedly seek reassurance

    that loved ones have not been harmed (Abramowitz et al., 2011; Frost & Steketee, 2002).

    Consistent with this formulation, research shows that the frequency or severity of

    obsessions in general is predicted by the strength of a person‘s beliefs about the

    significance of one‘s unwanted thoughts (e.g., ―Violent thoughts lead to dangerous

    deeds‖), and by beliefs about the importance of controlling such thoughts (e.g., ―If I don‘t

    drive the violent thoughts out of my mind, I will lose control and inflict harm on others‖) 

    (Taylor, Abramowitz, McKay, & Cuttler, 2012).

    5.2. Toward a Model of Musical Obsessions

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    24/61pted

        M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    22

    consequences of their unwanted, uncontrollable musical imagery. Therefore, individual

    differences in the frequency and duration of IMI seem likely to play some role in

    influencing the development of musical obsessions. However, even highly frequent and

     persistent IMI is insufficient by itself to account for musical obsessions. If a person

    interprets highly persistent IMI as an innocuous background soundtrack to daily life then

    IMI would not become a source of distress or lead to impairing avoidance behaviors or

    compulsions. Negative beliefs and appraisals seem important for transforming inherently

    innocuous IMI into musical obsessions.

     Negative appraisals of IMI can arise from beliefs that IMI is abnormal (e.g., a

    harbinger of mental deterioration), a sign that one lacks sufficient self-control (e.g., ―If I

    can‘t control my mind, then I might lose control of my behaviors‖), or a sign of some

    other aversive outcome (e.g., ―The music in my head interferes with my concentration; if

    I can‘t concentrate, then I might fail at school‖). Threat-related misinterpretations may

    lead to maladaptive coping responses that in turn increase the frequency of IMI. This

     pattern of maladaptive coping is suggested in the cases of musical obsessions reported by

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    25/61pted

        M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    23

    (Gomibuchi et al., 2000, p. 206)

    This account is similar to cognitive-behavioral accounts of OCD in general,

    concerning the paradoxical, self-defeating effects of attempting to suppress obsessional

    thoughts (Salkovskis, 1996).

    5.3. What Causes Individual Differences in IMI?

    Very little is known about the factors influencing individual differences in the

    frequency or persistence of IMI. This is an important gap in the research literature.

     Neural imaging and other forms of research suggest that auditory imagery involves the

    same brain areas as those involved in auditory perception (Hubbard, 2010; Zatorre &

    Halpern, 2005). These include the superior temporal gyrus, frontal and parietal lobes, and

    supplementary motor cortex (Hubbard, 2010; Janata, 2009). Levitin (2006) speculated

    that repetitive IMI occurs because ―the neural circuits representing the song get stuck in

    ‗playback mode‘‖ (p. 155). Given the multitude of structures involved in auditory

    imagery, it is likely that the interconnecting circuitry is complex. To better understand

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    26/61pted

        M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    24

    recurrent intrusions of verbal auditory imagery. There was suggestive evidence of this in

    our case series. One patient had musical obsessions along with auditory verbal intrusions

    consisting of the name of a dead friend that echoed in her mind (―Cabral, Cabral,

    Cabral‖; Fontenelle, 2008). In two other cases the person experienced intrusions

    consisting of lines of spoken poetry (Andrade & Rao, 1997; Saha, 2012). Further research

    is required to investigate whether musical obsessions are part of a general hyper-retrieval

    of auditory memories.

    6. Treatment

    The available database of cases provides leads as to how musical obsessions

    might be effectively treated, and provides a basis for developing treatment protocols that

    could be empirically evaluated in future research. Details of the treatments for the cases

    appear in the Appendix. The following is a summary of the salient points arising from the

    cases and the relevant literature.

    6.1. Assessment for Treatment Planning

    In assessing musical obsessions the clinician cannot rely solely on the Y-BOCS

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    27/61pted

        M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    25

    (Maršanić et al., 2011). In our case series, two patients were misdiagnosed as having a

     psychotic disorder (Maršanić et al., 2011; Nath et al., 2013). In neither case did the

     patient benefit from antipsychotics; in fact, there was evidence of symptom worsening.

    When musical obsessions were correctly diagnosed, treatment was changed to

    medications known to be efficacious for OCD (e.g., clomipramine, fluvoxamine) and the

     patients began to clinically improve. Accordingly, an accurate assessment of musical

    obsessions is important for planning appropriate treatment.

    6.2. Pharmacotherapy

    For musical obsessions in general, including those that were correctly diagnosed

    at the outset, our case studies suggest that patients tended not to benefit from

    antipsychotic medications (e.g., risperidone, clozapine, haloperidol). Such drugs may be

    effective in treating musical hallucinations (Evers & Ellger, 2004) and verbal auditory

    hallucinations associated with schizophrenia (Martinez, Marangell, & Martinez (2008).

    The case material also suggests that patients with musical obsessions are more likely to

     benefit from medications that are efficacious for OCD in general, such as selective

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    28/61pted

        M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    26

    without trying to suppress it. As had been reported for other obsessions, the effort the

     person puts into trying to suppress IMI is correlated with the frequency of IMI

    (Williamson & Müllensiefen, 2012). Reappraisal and acceptance strategies show promise

    for reducing distress and reduce maladaptive efforts at suppressing musical obsessions

    (Gomibuchi et al., 2000). A helpful coping statement might be: ―This is just music in my

    head; there is no need to dwell on it or fight it because it will eventually pass‖.

    6.4. Distraction

    Distraction-based treatments are not recommended for prototypic OC symptoms

    (Abramowitz et al., 2011). However, there are several lines of evidence suggesting that

    they can be useful in treating musical obsessions. Studies of normal IMI suggest that

    distraction can be useful. Retrospective surveys and prospective diary research indicates

    that people most commonly use distraction to terminate episodes of normal IMI (e.g.,

    keeping busy with whatever task is at hand, or listening to a competing piece of music;

    Beaman & Williams, 2010). In an experimental analogue study of normal volunteers,

    Hyman et al. (2013) found that normal IMI was disrupted when the person engaged in

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    29/61pted

        M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    27

    were conceptualized as similar to depressive ruminations, which can be successfully

    treated with distraction strategies (Teismann, Michalak, Willutzki, & Schulte, 2012). The

    additional component involving participants singing aloud the target song, with the

    clinician, may weaken the specificity of the IMI. That is, rather than only experiencing

    the obsession of the song as heard in its original form, the same melody now has

    associations with other sources. This is based on the assumption that the musical

    obsession has a singular association (song = aversive experience). With additional

    experiences of the melody under new conditions, the obsession has an expanded base of

    associations (song = aversive experience; song = clinical practice in patient and

    clinician‘s voice; song = other emotional experience). This expanded base is assumed to

    lead to greater flexibility in associations, derived from a relational frame theory (Dymond

    & Roche, 2013).

    6.5. Exposure and Response Prevention (ERP)

    The successful use of distraction or competing tasks in some cases of musical

    obsessions is quite different from treatments for other types of OC symptoms.

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    30/61pted

        M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    28

    suggest that ERP can be effective. Case reports are useful in that they suggest areas for

    further investigation. Future studies could directly compare distraction versus ERP as

    treatments for musical obsessions, and to investigate whether there are any variables.

    6.6. Summary

    There are several promising approaches to treating musical obsessions that merit

    further investigation in randomized, controlled studies. Musical obsessions may respond

    to medications and to cognitive-behavioral protocols used to treat OCD in general.

    However, some cases of musical obsessions have responded to treatments involving

    distraction or stimulus substitution. ERP, cognitive interventions, SSRIs, and

    clomipramine are first-line treatments for OCD in general, so it is reasonable to

    implement these interventions singly or in combination in the treatment of musical

    obsessions. If these interventions are unsuccessful then we recommend that the clinician

    reevaluate the diagnosis to determine whether the patient has musical obsessions or some

    other clinical condition such as a psychotic or neurological disorder. If the most likely

    diagnosis is musical obsessions, then distraction-based interventions could be considered.

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    31/61pted

        M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    29

    (b) musical obsessions lead to avoidance and compulsions in the same way that occurs

    for prototypic obsessions; and (c) musical obsessions are commonly comorbid with other

    types of OC symptoms, as is typically the case for OCD in general (APA, 2013).

    2. The previous literature has been limited mainly to a scattering of isolated case

    reports, which has been taken as suggesting that musical obsessions are rare. However,

    we were able to readily identify a large number of unpublished cases, obtained from

    clinicians with expertise with OCD. It may be that musical obsessions are more common

    than previously recognized.

    3. Musical obsessions may be under-diagnosed and under-treated because

    contemporary assessment methods do not sufficiently probe for such phenomena, and

    clinicians may be unfamiliar with musical obsessions. Musical obsessions are sometimes

    misdiagnosed as psychotic phenomena, which has led to inappropriate treatment (i.e.,

    antipsychotic medication). An accurate diagnosis is important for planning appropriate

    treatment.

    4. Musical obsessions differ from visual obsessions in that the content of the

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    32/61pte  d    M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    30

    etiologic factors (environment and genetic) but also are shaped by factors that influence

    all OC symptoms (Taylor, 2011).

    5. IMI is common and yet musical obsessions are comparatively rare. Building on

     previous theorizing on OCD (e.g., Frost & Steketee, 2002), we propose a set of testable

    hypotheses about the etiology of musical obsessions. Factors that influence the

     persistence or frequency of IMI may play a role but may be insufficient because

     persistent, frequent IMI is not always distressing. Consistent with contemporary cognitive

    models of OCD (Frost & Steketee, 2002), we propose that the person‘s beliefs and

    appraisals of IMI play an important role in the etiology and maintenance of musical

    obsessions. If a person appraises the IMI as threatening (e.g., as an indication of a life

    threatening neurological problem), then he or she will become preoccupied and distressed

    with the musical imagery. This may lead to functional impairment (e.g., failure to go to

    the workplace because ringtones might be heard, or failure to fulfill household

    responsibilities because of avoidance of supermarkets, in which background music is

    often played).

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    33/61pte  d    M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    31

    obsessions, based on the largest set of case descriptions ever assembled. Our review is

    comprehensive in that we sought to cover all the relevant literature, including, for

    comparison with musical obsessions, the literatures on IMI in general and on visual

    obsessional imagery. In terms of weaknesses, the cases in our review were almost

    entirely clinical (treatment-seeking) individuals, and so it is unclear whether the findings

    generalize to people who suffer from musical obsessions but do not seek treatment. A

    further limitation is that there have been few studies comparing patients with musical

    obsessions to other patient groups, and treatments have not been evaluated by means of

    randomized controlled trials. A further limitation is that there have been few large-scale

    empirical studies of IMI, and no empirical studies of the etiology of musical obsessions.

    These limitations are unavoidable, given the lack of empirical attention to the neglected

    clinical phenomena of musical obsessions. Despite these limitations, our review presents

    the best available clinical description of musical obsessions, along with testable

    hypotheses about etiology and treatment.

    7.3. Future Directions

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    34/61pte  d    M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    32

    obsessions. Further research is also needed to investigate the cognitive-behavioral and

    neurobiological bases of normal IMI in general, and musical obsessions in particular.

    Research is also needed to investigate the promising treatments identified in the case

    studies, including the distraction-based interventions, which are quite different from

    exposure-based empirically supported treatments for other types of obsessive-compulsive

     phenomena.

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    35/61pte  d    M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    33

    References

    Abramowitz, J. S., Deacon, B. J., & Whiteside, S. P. H. (2011). Exposure therapy for

    anxiety: Principles and practice. New York: Guilford.

    Ahuja, N. (2001). Two cases of musical obsessions. Proceedings of the 53rd Annual

     National Conference of the Indian Psychiatric Society. Pune, India.

    Akhtar, S., Wig, N. N., Varma, V. K., Pershad, I., & Verma, S. K. (1975). A

     phenomenological analysis of symptoms in obsessive-compulsive neurosis.

     British Journal of Psychiatry, 127 , 342-348.

    American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental

    disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: Author.

    Andrade, C., & Rao, N. S. K. (1997). Musical obsessions: A case report. Indian Journal

    of Psychiatry, 39, 178-180.

    Bailes, F. A. (2007). The prevalence and nature of imagined music in the everyday lives

    of music students. Psychology of Music, 35, 555-570.

    Beaman, C. P., & Williams, T. I. (2010). Earworms (―stuck song syndrome‖): Towards a

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    36/61pte  d    M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    34

    obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 10,

    375.

    Chauhan, N., Shah, R., & Grover, S. (2010). Obsessive auditory imagery: A case report.

     African Journal of Psychiatry, 13, 313.

    de Silva, P. (1986). Obsessional-compulsive imagery. Behaviour Research and Therapy,

    24, 333-350.

    Di Dio, A. S., Fields, M. C., & Rowan, A. J. (2007). Palinacousis - Auditory

     perseveration: Two cases and a review of the literature. Epilepsia, 48, 1801-1806.

    Dymond, S., & Roche, B. (2013). Advances in relational frame theory. Oakland, CA:

    Context Press.

    Ebbinghaus, H. (1885). Über das Gedächtnis: Untersuchungen zur experimentellen

     Psychologie. Leipzig: von Duncker & Humblot.

    El-Mallakh, R. S., & Walker, K. L. (2010). Hallucinations, pseudohallucinations, and

     parahallucinations. Psychiatry, 73, 34-42.

    Emmelkamp, P. M. G., & Kwee, K. G. (1977). Obsessional ruminations: A comparison

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    37/61pte  d    M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    35

    Greece.

    Foa, E. B., Huppert, J. D., Leiberg, S., Langner, R., Kichic, R., Hajcak, G., … 

    Salkovskis, P. M. (2002). The obsessive-compulsive inventory: Development and

    validation of a short version. Psychological Assessment, 14, 485-496.

    Fontenelle, L. F. (2008). Pareidolias in obsessive-compulsive disorder: Neglected

    symptoms that may respond to serotonin reuptake inhibitors. Neurocase, 14, 414-

    418.

    Frost, R. O., & Steketee, G. (Eds.). (2002). Cognitive approaches to obsessions and

    compulsions: Theory, assessment and treatment . Oxford: Elsevier.

    Gomibuchi, T., Gomibuchi, K., Akiyama, T., Tsuda, H., & Hayakawa, T. (2000).

    Obsession of hearing music: From the viewpoint of Morita theory. Psychiatry and

    Clinical Neurosciences, 54, 203-206.

    Goodman, W. K., Rasmussen, S. A., Price, L. H., Mazure, C., Heninger, G. R., &

    Charney, D. S. (1989). Manual for the Yale-Brown obsessive compulsive scale 

    (rev.). New Haven, CT: Connecticut Mental Health Center.

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    38/61pte  d    M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    36

    Greece.

    Hermesh, H., Konas, S., Shiloh, R., Reuven, D., Marom, S., Weizman, A., & … Gross-

    Isseroff, R. (2004). Musical hallucinations: Prevalence in psychotic and

    nonpsychotic outpatients. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 65, 191-197.

    Hubbard, T. L. (2010). Auditory imagery: Empirical findings. Psychological Bulletin,

    136, 302-329.

    Hyman, I. E., Burland, N. K., Duskin, H. M., Cook, M. C., Roy, C. M., McGrath, J. C., ...

    Roundhill, R. F. (2013). Going Gaga: Investigating, creating, and manipulating

    the song stuck in my head. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 27 , 204-215.

    Jacobs, L., Feldman, M., Diamond, S. P., & Bender, M. B. (1973). Palinacousis:

    Persistent or recurring auditory sensations. Cortex, 9, 275-287.

    Janata P. (2009). The neural architecture of music-evoked autobiographical memory

    Cerebral Cortex, 19, 2579-2594.

    Janet, P. (1903). Les obsessions et la psychasthénie. Paris: Alcan.

    Jaspers, K. (1913). Allgemeine Psychopathologie gie: Ein Leitfaden für Studierende,

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    39/61pte  d    M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    37

    Levitin, D. J. (2006). This is your brain on music. New York: Dutton.

    Liikkanen, L. A. (2012a). Inducing involuntary musical imagery: An experimental study.

     Musicae Scientiae, 16 , 236-256.

    Liikkanen, L. A. (2012b). Musical activities predispose to involuntary musical imagery.

     Psychology of Music, 40, 236-256.

    Liikkanen, L. A. (2012c). New directions for understanding involuntary musical imagery.

    In E. Cambouropoulos, C. Tsougras, P. Mavromatis, & K. Pastiadis (Eds.),

     Proceedings of the 12th international conference on music perception and

    cognition (pp. 587-592). Thessaloniki, Greece.

    Lipton, M. G., Brewin, C. R., Linke, S., & Halperin, J. (2010). Distinguishing features of

    intrusive images in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders,

    24, 816-822.

    Maršanić, V. B., Aukst-Margetić, B., Grgić, N., & Kušmić, E. (2011). Misdiagnosis and

    exacerbation of unusual obsessive-compulsive disorder presentation with

    Risperidone and Clozapine in an adolescent girl –  a case report. Collegium

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    40/61pte  d    M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    38

    Mendhekar, D. N., & Andrade, C. (2009). Musical obsession: Repeated auditory imagery

    of cell phone ring tone. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 63, 591-592.

    Miguel, E. C., Leckman, J. F., Rauch, S., do Rosario-Campos, M. C., Hounie, A. G.,

    Mercadante, M. T., … Pauls, D. L. (2005). Obsessive-compulsive disorder

     phenotypes: Implications for genetic studies. Molecular Psychiatry, 10, 258-275.

    Mohamed, W., Ahuja, N., & Shah, A. (2012). Palinacousis –  Evidence to suggest a post-

    ictal phenomenon. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 317 , 6-12.

    Møller, A. R. (2011). Misophonia, phonophobia, and ―exploding head‖ syndrome. In A.

    R. Møller (Ed.), Textbook of tinnitus (pp. 25-27). New York: Springer Science +

    Business.

    Mueller, A., Mitchell, J. E., Crosby, R. D., Glaesmer, H., & de Zwaan, M. (2009). The

     prevalence of compulsive hoarding and its association with compulsive buying in

    a German population-based sample. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47 , 705-

    709.

    Murphy, T. K., Kurlan, R., & Leckman, J. (2010). The immunobiology of Tourette‘s

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    41/61pte  d    M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    39

    the obsessive belief questionnaire and interpretation of intrusions inventory –  Part

    2: Factor analyses and testing of a brief version. Behaviour Research andTherapy, 43, 1527-1542.

    Oyebode, F. (2001). Sims’ symptoms in the mind: An introduction to descriptive

     psychopathology (4th ed). Philadelphia, PA: Elsevier Saunders.

    Pfizer, N., & Andrade, C. (1999). Isolated musical obsessions. Indian Journal of

     Psychiatry, 41, 77-78.

    Podoll, K. (2010). Musical palinacousis as an aura symptom in persistent aura without

    infarction. In F. C. Rose (Ed.), Neurology of music (pp. 221-237). London:

    Imperial College Press.

    Praharaj, S. K., Goyal, N., Sarkar, S., Bagati, D., Sinha, P., & Sinha, V. K. (2009).

    Musical obsession or pseudohallucination: Electrophysiological standpoint.

     Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 63, 230-234.

    Rapoport, J. L. (1980). The boy who couldn’t stop washing: The experience and

    treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder. New York: Dutton.

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    42/61pte  d    M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    40

     Mental Health, and Neurosciences, 5, 42-47.

    Sedman, G. (1966). A comparative study of pseudohallucinations, imagery and true

    hallucinations. British Journal of Psychiatry, 112, 9-17.

    Speckens, A. E. M., Hackmann, A., Ehlers, A., & Cuthbert, B. (2007). Intrusive images

    and memories of earlier adverse events in patients with obsessive compulsive

    disorder. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38, 411-422.

    Stafford, T. (2012). Earworms: Why catchy tunes get trapped in our heads. Retrieved

    July 16, 2013, from http://www.bbc.com/future/story/20120411-why-do-songs-

    stick-in-our-heads/all

    Taylor, S. (2011). Etiology of obsessions and compulsions: A meta-analysis and narrative

    review of twin studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 1361-1372.

    Taylor, S., Abramowitz, J. S., McKay, D., & Cuttler, C. (2012). Cognitive approaches to

    understanding obsessive-compulsive and related disorders. In G. Steketee (Ed.),

    Oxford handbook of obsessive compulsive and spectrum disorders (pp. 233-250).

     New York: Oxford University Press.

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    43/61pte  d    M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    41

    Cambouropoulos, C. Tsougras, P. Mavromatis, & K. Pastiadis (Eds.),

     Proceedings of the 12

    th

     international conference of music perception andcognition (pp. 1124-1132). Thessaloniki, Greece.

    Zatorre, R. J., & Halpern, A. R. (2005). Mental concerts: Musical imagery and auditory

    cortex. Neuron, 47 , 9-12.

    Zungu-Dirwayi, N., Hugo, F., van Heerden, B. B., & Stein, D. J. (1999). Are musical

    obsessions a temporal lobe phenomenon? Journal of Neuropsychiatry and

    Clinical Neurosciences, 11, 398-400.

    Appendix

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    44/61pte  d    M  a  n

       u  s  c  r   i   p   t

    1

    Appendix of Supplementary Materials for:

    Musical Obsessions: A Comprehensive Review of Neglected Clinical Phenomena

    Correspondence: Steven Taylor, Ph.D., Department of Psychiatry, 2255 Wesbrook Mall,

    University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada, V6T 2A1. Email:

    [email protected], Tel: 011-1-604-785-7558, Fax: 011-1-604-822-7756.

    p

    mailto:[email protected]:[email protected]:[email protected]

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    45/61

    Page 44 of 60

       A  c  c  e   p   t  e  d

        M  a  n   u

      s  c  r   i   p   t

    2

    Table A1. Previously published case studies of musical obsessions.

    Case Country Age Sex Occupation Musical

    Obsession(s)

    Duration of

    Musical Obsession(s)

     Notes, Including a Description of Other

    Clinical Features (Current or Past)

    1. 

    Ahuja (2001) India 24 F Trainee physician

    Tunes from Hindisongs.

    Two episodes; mostrecent was 8 months

     No other OC symptoms. OCPD traits. Onsetwhile studying for exams.

    2.  Ahuja (2001) India 33 F Homemaker Lyrics and tunes

    from Hindi songs.

    18 months Washing, sexual obsessions. OC personality

    traits. Marked impairment in daily life.

    3.  Akhtar et al.

    (1975)

    India 23 F Student A tune popular at

    the time.

    -- --

    4.  Andrade &

    Rao (1997)

    India 30 M Musician Some musical

     passages were more

    troubling that others,

     particularly Ravel’s

    Bolero, a Chopin

    nocturne, an operatic piece by Verdi, andsome of the person’s

    own compositions.

    Over 5 years Recognized the musical imagery as his own

    thoughts but regarded them as abnormal. Tried

    to resist with little success, although

    sometimes he was successful by concentrating

    on other melodies. Also reported intrusive

    verses of poetry, and checking and orderingcompulsions. OCPD traits. No evidence ofneurological problems.

    5.  Berg (1953):

    Summary

    of 10 cases

    USA Adult 40%F 70%

    employed,

    30%

    students

    Popular songs from

    the 1920s-1950s.

    -- All patients were in counseling for various

    stress-related problems unrelated to musical

    obsessions (e.g., distress regarding financial

     problems).

    6.  Cameron &

    Wasielewski

    (1990)

    USA 24 M Student Any music he had

    recently heard.

    2 years No other psychopathology. Musical

    obsessions started 1 month after he received a

     blow to the head, but a comprehensive

    evaluation, including EEG, revealed no

    neurological abnormalities.7.  Fontenelle

    (2008)

    Brazil 38 F Homemaker Various popular

    songs.

    -- Distressing pareidolias (illusory images seen

    out of shapes; e.g., seeing gorillas in the floor

    tiles). Auditory and visual images of a dead

    friend (auditory imagery of his name and

    visual imagery of the imagined funeral).

    Aggressive obsessions, washing, ordering, and

    hoarding rituals. Depression,

    depersonalization and derealization. Normal

    p

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    46/61

    Page 45 of 60

       A  c  c  e   p   t  e  d

        M  a  n   u

      s  c  r   i   p   t

    3

    EEG.

    8.  Gomibuchi et

    al. (2000)

     –  Case 1

    Japan 19 M Student Various melodies he

    had recently heard.

    Several

    months

    Stress-related onset (studying for exams).

    Tried to suppress the musical imagery, with

    little success. Musical intrusions were most

    frequent and distressing when he was tryinghard to concentrate while studying. OCPD

    traits (highly perfectionistic). Misophonia

    (low tolerance for sounds; minor sounds

    evoked distress).

    9.  Gomibuchi et

    al. (2000)

     –  Case 2

    Japan 18 M Student Music he had heard

    to the previous day.

    Several

    months

    Stress-related onset (studying for exams).

    Used to listen to music while studying because

    it helped block out extraneous noises. Then

    tried to study without listening to music but

     began experiencing musical obsessions.

    Musical intrusions were most frequent and

    distressing when he was trying hard to

    concentrate while studying. OCPD traits(highly perfectionistic), mild checking

    compulsions.

    10.  Maršanić et al.

    (2011)

    Croatia 17 F Student Popular songs and

     jingles.

    One month

    (approx.)

    Grades at school dropped significantly due to

    musical obsessions. She often attempted to

    suppress the symptoms but was unable to do

    so. Intense anxiety, insomnia,

    depersonalization, derealization, along with

    harming and sexual obsessions. Neurological

    examination, including EEG, was normal.

    Initially misdiagnosed as being acutely

     psychotic.

    11. 

    Matsui et al.

    (2003)

     –  Case 1

    Japan 20 M Student Popular songs,

     jingles, and other

    well-known

    melodies.

    2 years Intrusions were particularly troublesome when

    he was studying for exams. He described the

    symptoms as “a broken tape recorder”

    repeatedly producing the same songs and

    music all day long. It was monotonous and

    interfered with his normal routine, academic

    functioning, and usual social activities or

    relationships. Exacerbated by stress (studying

    for exams). Coping responses to musical

    p

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    47/61

    Page 46 of 60

       A  c  c  e   p   t  e  d

        M  a  n   u

      s  c  r   i   p   t

    4

    obsessions consisted of covert compulsions

    (suppression, neutralization). OCPD traits

    (perfectionistic), anxious, and depressed

    (dysthymic disorder). The more he worried

    about, and tried to suppress, the musicalimagery, the more persistent it became.

    Avoided classes and social activities due to

    musical obsessions. Experienced

    depersonalization. Initially misdiagnosed as

    having schizophrenia. Neurological exam

    including EEG was normal. ENT normal.

    12.  Matsui et al.

    (2003)

     –  Case 2

    Japan 28 M Retail sales Current and old

     popular songs.

    2 years Premorbid “tender and obedient” character.

    Onset after he quit his job due to interpersonal

    conflict (stress-related). He often attempted to

    suppress the symptoms by thinking about

    another melody (i.e., suppress, neutralize),

    although he usually was unable to do so.Obsessions worsened when he started living

    alone. Neurological exam normal, including

    normal EEG. Comorbid major depressive

    disorder.

    13.  Mendhekar &

    Andrade

    (2009)

    India 22 M Student Various cell phone

    ring tones, occurring

    6-8 hrs/day.

    4 months Feared and avoided exposure to ring tones. He

    wore earplugs, used distraction by thinking of

    other tunes, kept his phone in silent mode, and

    asked others to do the same, avoided places he

    might hear the ringing of cell phones. Prior to

    onset of musical obsession he had enjoyed

    downloading and changing ring tones on his

     phone. He would devote 2-3 hrs every 2-3

    days to this activity (i.e., had been spending a

    lot of time listening to ringtones). History of

    counting, checking, and washing. At the time

    of presentation he had mild comorbid

    checking. Musical obsessions interfered with

    his ability to study and became depressed as a

    result.

    14.   Nath et al. India 22 M -- Recently heard 3 years Attempted to resist the musical imagery but

    p

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    48/61

    Page 47 of 60

       A  c  c  e   p   t  e  d

        M  a  n   u

      s  c  r   i   p   t

    5

    (2013) tunes. was unsuccessful. Vocal tics, washing,

    harming obsessions, ordering, hoarding,

    sexual obsessional images. Initially

    misdiagnosed as having schizophrenia. No

    history of neurological problems.15.  Pfizer &

    Andrade

    (1999)

    India 25 F Homemaker A single Tamil film

    song running

    through her head for

    8 hrs/day

    Arose suddenly 4 days

     prior to seeking

    treatment, along with a

    sore throat and fever,

     but did not abate when

    the infection was

    successfully treated

    with erythromycin.

    Her attempts at controlling the intrusion were

    unsuccessful. No past history of

     psychopathology. Musical obsessions

     persisted despite no evidence of a general

    medical condition. Reported feeling

    depersonalized because of the obsession.

    16.  Praharaj et al.

    (2009)

    India 21 M -- Sections of music

    from various

     popular Hindi films,

    lasting from 2-45min per episode,

    occurring 30-35

    times/day, nearly

    every day.

    Occasionally a new

    song he heard

    replaced an older

    one.

    5 years Tunes commonly intruded when he was alone.

    He was partially successful in temporarily

     banishing them by distraction such as

    engaging in games or conversations, but theintrusions returned. Washing, checking,

    doubting, intrusive visual obsessions, severe

    depression. Normal physical examination.

     Normal EEG.

    17.  Rapoport

    (1980)

    USA 16 M Student Popular music,

    movie scores.

    7 years Past history of checking, counting, washing,

    and superstitious rituals (only even numbers

    were acceptable). Was in therapy for 4 years

    for anxiety and adjustment problems but never

    disclosed his musical obsessions. Efforts at

    suppressing imagery was unsuccessful.

    Musical obsessions greatly interfered with

    everyday functioning. Normal CT.

    18.  Rapoport

    (1980)

    USA 60 M Farmer Six or more notes of

    a fiddle tune (“a dull

    short jig”). 

    31 years Kept the musical obsession secret from others.

    His spouse observed that he looked

     preoccupied while he was experiencing the

    obsessions. No evidence of neurological or

    p

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    49/61

    Page 48 of 60

       A  c  c  e   p   t  e  d

        M  a  n   u

      s  c  r   i   p   t

    6

    audiologic problems, and no evidence of

     psychosis. Experienced the tune as a constant

    unpleasantness, bore, and irritation. Described

    it as “living my life through the noise.” 

    19. 

    Saha (2012) India 30 M -- Music that he orothers had

    composed,

    associated with

    intrusive images of

    him humming the

    tunes.

    4 years Attempted to resist with little success.Distraction with other melodies was

    successful, at least in the short term. Intrusive

    verses of poetry, along with checking,

    ordering symptoms.

    20.  Zungu-

    Dirwayi et al.

    (1999) –  Case

    1

    South

    Africa

    59 F -- Various tunes. 20 years. Developed in

    the context of major

    depression following

    her husband’s death 20

    years previously.

    Obsessions persistedafter depression abated.

    Tried to get rid of the musical obsessions by

    substituting them with other thoughts or tunes.

     Normal EEG.

    21.  Zungu-

    Dirwayi et al.

    (1999) –  Case

    2

    South

    Africa

    29 F -- Unspecified tune. Intrusive tune

    developed 5 months

    after a closed head

    injury. Intrusion had

     persisted for a month at

    the time of assessment.

    Unconscious for 4 days due to head injury.

    One month of retrograde amnesia. Post-injury

     personality changes (increased aggressiveness

    and apathy) with little insight as to the

    changes. CT scan was normal. Unsuccessful

    attempts at thought suppression.

    -- = Not reported. Gomibuchi et al. (2000) presented, for illustrative purposes, 2 of 5 of their cases of musical obsessions (4 M, 1 F, ages 18-22). References to all

    cited articles appear in the reference list of the main article.

    p

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    50/61

    Page 49 of 60

       A  c  c  e   p   t  e  d

        M  a  n   u

      s  c  r   i   p   t

    7

    Table A2. Unpublished cases of musical obsessions from the authors’ clinical files. 

    Case Country Age Sex Occupation Musical

    Obsession(s)

    Duration of

    Musical

    Obsession(s)

     Notes, Including Details of Other Clinical

    Features (Current or Past)

    1.  BCR –  Case 1

    USA 21 M Student Songs on hisiPod, which

    were mostly

     popular songs.

    If someone

    was to sing a

    song in

     passing, and

    he knew the

    song, it would

    also become a

    musicalobsession.

    -- Would hear a song involuntarily playing in hishead and then need to play it out or listen to it in

    full before moving on –  otherwise it would be

    “stuck” all day. Washing, hoarding, doubting,

    obsessions (somatic, aggressive, sexual).

    Anorexia nervosa, mood disorder NOS, GAD.

    2.  BCR –  

    Case 2

    USA 59 M Unemployed

    (former

    cleric)

     Numerous

    types of songs

    (e.g., jingles,

    Christmas

    songs,

    hymnals,

     popular music

    on the radio).

    Intermittently

    since teens.

    Fear that a song will get stuck in his head and

    then he will be unable to function. No other OC

    symptoms. Recurrent, moderate major depressive

    disorder.

    3.  CA India 23 M Unemployed Not reported. Intermittently,

    sometimes

    lasting a day

    or more.

    Child-onset OCD and comorbid bipolar disorder,

    for which the patient was in a depressive phase at

    the time of assessment and treatment. OC

    symptoms included musical and visual

    obsessions. The latter consisted of sexual and

    violent obsessions.

    4.  DM –  

    Case 1

    USA 9 M Student A pop song

    that was

    heavily played

    on the radio.

    2 months Avoided exposure to music for fear of developing

    new intrusive song. Sexual and aggressive

    obsessions, along with compulsions to confess

    unwanted thoughts. Past history of separation

    anxiety and mild attention difficulties.

    p

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    51/61

    Page 50 of 60

       A  c  c  e   p   t  e  d

        M  a  n   u

      s  c  r   i   p   t

    8

    5.  DM –  

    Case 2

    USA 37 F Lawyer Heavy metal

    songs

    4 months Washing, checking. Past history of abuse of a

     prescribed anxiolytic.

    6.  DM –  

    Case 3

    USA 49 M Lawyer Popular songs

    and TV

    themes.

    Intermittently

    for 2 years

    Violent imagery and harming obsessions. Past

    history of depression.

    7.  DS –  

    Case 1

    Canada 25 M Student Bach, various

     piano music.

    -- OCPD traits (perfectionistic). Symmetry,

    ordering, and counting.

    8.  DS –  

    Case 2

    Canada 67 M Retired

     businessman

    Choral music. -- Checking and religious obsessions.

    9.  DS –  

    Case 3

    Canada 43 F Homemaker Lullabies. -- Washing.

    10.  DS –  

    Case 4

    Canada 83 M Retired

    lawyer

    Funeral dirges. 3 months.

    Musical

    obsession

    started after

    the death of

    his wife.

    Depression, ordering.

    11. 

    DS –  

    Case 5

    Canada 32 F Retail sales Any song she

    recently heard.

    -- Musical obsessions exacerbated by stress. OCPD

    traits (perfectionistic). Ordering rituals.

    12.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 1

    Brazil 22 F Student Not reported. 8 years Washing, ordering, hoarding, and obsessions

    (aggressive, sexual, religious).

    13.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 2

    Brazil 25 M Factory

    worker

     Not reported. 10 years Washing, ordering, hoarding, and obsessions

    (aggressive, sexual, religious).

    14.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 3

    Brazil 28 F Office

    supervisor

     Not reported. 14 years Washing, ordering, hoarding.

    15.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 4

    Brazil 31 F Office

    assistant

     Not reported. 16 years Washing, ordering, hoarding, and obsessions

    (aggressive, sexual, religious).

    16.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 5

    Brazil 21 M Computer

    industry

     Not reported. 4 years Hoarding, ordering, and obsessions (aggressive,

    sexual, religious).

    17.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 6

    Brazil 31 F Architect Not reported. 6 years Washing, ordering, and obsessions (aggressive,

    sexual, religious).

    p

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    52/61

    Page 51 of 60

       A  c  c  e   p   t  e  d

        M  a  n   u

      s  c  r   i   p   t

    9

    18.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 7

    Brazil 37 F Business

    consultant

     Not reported. 19 years Washing, ordering, and obsessions (aggressive,

    sexual, religious).

    19.  ECM&

    ADM –  Case 8

    Brazil 45 F Researcher Not reported. 20 years Washing and ordering.

    20.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 9

    Brazil 34 F Advertising Not reported. 23 years Washing, hoarding, ordering, and obsessions

    (aggressive, sexual, religious).

    21.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 10

    Brazil 22 F Telemarketer  Not reported. 1 year Washing, ordering, and aggressive obsessions.

    22.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 11

    Brazil 60 F Homemaker Not reported. 53 years Washing, ordering, and obsessions (aggressive,

    sexual, religious).

    23.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 12

    Brazil 25 F Lawyer Not reported. 13 years Washing, hoarding, ordering, and obsessions

    (aggressive, sexual, religious).

    24.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 13

    Brazil 58 F Retired Not reported. 18 years Washing, ordering, and obsessions (aggressive,

    sexual, religious).

    25.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 14

    Brazil 42 F Independentlywealthy*

     Not reported. 24 years Washing, hoarding, ordering, and obsessions

    (sexual and religious)

    26.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 15

    Brazil 46 F Office

    worker

     Not reported. 20 years Washing, ordering, and aggressive obsessions.

    27.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 16

    Brazil 54 F Homemaker Not reported. 24 years Washing, hoarding, ordering, and obsessions

    (aggressive, sexual, and religious)

    28.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 17

    Brazil 43 F Homemaker Not reported. 23 years Washing, ordering, and obsessions (aggressive,

    sexual, and religious)

    29.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 18

    Brazil 32 F Secretary Not reported. 20 years Washing, hoarding, ordering, and aggressive

    obsessions.

    30.  ECM& Brazil 60 F Office Not reported. 45 years Washing, hoarding, ordering, and obsessions

    pt 10

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    53/61

    Page 52 of 60

       A  c  c  e   p   t  e  d

        M  a  n   u

      s  c  r   i   p   t

    10

    ADM –  

    Case 19

    worker (aggressive, sexual, and religious)

    31.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 20

    Brazil 53 F Homemaker Not reported. 33 years Washing, hoarding, ordering, and obsessions

    (aggressive, sexual, and religious)

    32.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 21

    Brazil 27 F Marketing Not reported. 22 years Symmetry

    33.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 22

    Brazil 25 F Lawyer Not reported. 14 years Washing, hoarding, symmetry

    34.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 23

    Brazil 60 M Retired Not reported. 10 years Symmetry

    35.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 24

    Brazil 33 M Lawyer Not reported. 26 years Washing, hoarding, symmetry, and obsessions

    (aggressive, sexual, and religious)

    36. 

    ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 25

    Brazil 22 F Student Not reported. 3 years Washing, symmetry, and obsessions (aggressive,

    sexual, and religious)

    37.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 26

    Brazil 25 F Admin. Not reported. 10 years Washing, symmetry

    38.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 27

    Brazil 68 M Admin. Not reported. 53 years Washing, symmetry, and obsessions (aggressive,

    sexual, and religious)

    39.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 28

    Brazil 18 M Student Not reported. 10 years Washing, symmetry

    40. 

    ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 29

    Brazil 26 M Admin. Not reported. 6 years Symmetry

    41.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 30

    Brazil 29 F Hair stylist Not reported. 14 years Washing, hoarding, symmetry, and obsessions

    (aggressive, sexual, and religious)

    pt 11

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    54/61

    Page 53 of 60

       A  c  c  e   p   t  e  d

        M  a  n   u

      s  c  r   i   p   t

    11

    42.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 31

    Brazil 48 M Teacher Not reported. 1 year Symmetry, and obsessions (aggressive, sexual,

    and religious)

    43.  ECM&

    ADM –  Case 32

    Brazil 21 M Student Not reported. 6 years Symmetry, and obsessions (sexual and religious)

    44.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 33

    Brazil 42 F Diplomat Not reported. 25 years Symmetry and violent obsessions

    45.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 34

    Brazil 27 M Communi-

    cations

     Not reported. 17 years Washing, symmetry

    46.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 35

    Brazil 20 F Teacher Not reported. 8 years Washing, hoarding, symmetry, and obsessions

    (aggressive, sexual, and religious)

    47.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 36

    Brazil 38 F Homemaker Not reported. 23 years Washing

    48.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 37

    Brazil 36 M Dentist Not reported. 22 years Washing, hoarding, symmetry, and obsessions

    (aggressive, sexual, and religious)

    49.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 38

    Brazil 27 F Admin. Not reported. 17 years Symmetry, and obsessions (aggressive, sexual,

    and religious)

    50.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 39

    Brazil 33 M Railway

    worker

     Not reported. 2 years Washing, hoarding, symmetry, and obsessions

    (aggressive, sexual, and religious)

    51.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 40

    Brazil 30 F Saleswoman Not reported. 1 year Washing, hoarding, symmetry

    52.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 41

    Brazil 29 M Technical

    support

     Not reported. 7 years Washing, hoarding, symmetry, and obsessions

    (aggressive, sexual, and religious)

    53.  ECM&

    ADM –  

    Case 42

    Brazil 24 F Actress Not reported. 9 years Symmetry and violent obsessions

    54.  JSK& Republic 24 M Student Korean pop 4 years Musical obsessions were accompanied by mild

    pt 12

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    55/61

    Page 54 of 60

       A  c  c  e   p   t  e  d

        M  a  n   u

      s  c  r   i   p   t

    12

    MJH –  

    Case 1

    of Korea song. depression. Symptoms were aggravated while

    studying for exams. Patient also had aggressive,

    sexual obsessions and related checking as

    concurrent symptoms.

    55. 

    JSK&MJH –  

    Case 2

    Republicof Korea 21 M Student Korean popsong. 3 years Musical obsessions started after conflicts withfriends at school. Patient also had aggressive

    obsessions without compulsion, somatic

    obsessions, pathological doubt and idea of

    reference as concurrent symptoms.

    56.  JSK&

    MJH –  

    Case 3

    Republic

    of Korea

    30 M Unemployed Korean pop

    song.

    9 months After listening to music, the melody of music

    continued for an hour. Due to the somatic

    obsessions and related idea of reference he had

    cosmetic surgery on his nose. Comorbid major

    depressive disorder and social anxiety disorder

    57.  JSK&

    MJH –  

    Case 4

    Republic

    of Korea

    20 M Student Korean pop

    song.

    4 years When he experienced musical obsessions, he also

    had to perform his activities according to the

    rhythm of them. Patient also had repeating,

    checking, contamination, cleaning, perfectionism

    and impulsivity as concurrent symptoms.

    Comorbid bipolar disorder NOS.

    58.  JSK&

    MJH –  

    Case 5

    Republic

    of Korea

    18 M Student Korean pop

    song.

    6 years Korean pops which he usually listens to replayed

    in his head for several hours. These musical

    obsessions did not disturb his concentration or

    activities of daily life. Patient also had

     pathological doubt, checking, aggressive and

    sexual obsessions as concurrent symptoms.

    59.  JSK&

    MJH –  

    Case 6

    Republic

    of Korea

    31 F Office

    worker

    Korean pop

    song.

    9 years Musical obsessions started after job-related stress.

    The melody of the music that she heard in the

    morning replayed all day long. She began binge-

    eating to avoid the stress due to the musical

    obsessions. Recently musical obsessions were

    aggravated by the conflict with a colleague at

    work and she couldn’t concentrate on her work.

    Patient also had checking and hoarding

    compulsions. Comorbid bulimia nervosa, alcohol

    abuse and depression.

    pt 13

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    56/61

    Page 55 of 60

       A  c  c  e   p   t  e  d

        M  a  n   u

      s  c  r   i   p   t

    13

    60.  KO Canada 33 M Unemployed Sad songs

    replaying in

    his head. He

    feared that

    they would provoke bad

    events in

    reality

    -- Duration of up to 12 hrs/day. Onset frequently

    associated with sad memories. Sometimes the

    intrusive musical imagery was so intense that he

    difficulty following conversations. He consulted a

     psychiatrist for fears that he might haveschizophrenia. Comorbid symmetry and

    superstitious rituals, and body focused repetitive

     behaviors (skin picking and nail biting).

    61.  KO Canada 32 M Student &

    teacher

    Parts of

    various songs

    intrude

    randomly

    -- Songs can repeat in a loop up to 4hrs per day

    every day, interferes with work and writing,

    incapable of concentrating. No particular trigger

     but only occur when stressed.Comorbid harming

    obsessions.

    62.  LF Brazil 29 F Student Any recently

    heard song

    “with chorus” 

    10 years History of obsessions with aggressive content at

    age 9, which disappeared soon afterwards. At age

    14, she had her first panic attack and developed,

    since then, chronic panic disorder with

    agoraphobia. OCD, including sexual/religious and

    aggressive obsessions reappeared acutely at age

    19, together with the onset of musical obsessions.

    For quite awhile, musical obsessions remained

    her most significant symptom. They were worse

    when she was either relaxed or very stressed. Of

    note, she did not endorse any type of overt or

    covert compulsions. At her worse, her Y-BOCS

    obsessions score was 14 and BDI 26. She had her

    first depressive episode at age 19, 2 months

     before the onset of OCD. In addition, she

    developed chronic and persistent

    depersonalization disorder (not ascribed to OCD

    or panic attacks) since age 23.

    63.  LH USA 24 F Admin.

    assistant and

    graduate

    student.

    Any recently

    heard song.

    Intermittent

    episodes since

    adolescence.

    Musical obsession was a negative side effect o f a

    coping strategy of singing to herself to distract

    from other obsessions. Began hearing songs

    whenever not engaged in focused conversation.

    Musical obsession caused significant interference,

    especially at night, contributing to insomnia.

    pt 14

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    57/61

    Page 56 of 60

       A  c  c  e   p   t  e  d

        M  a  n   u

      s  c  r   i   p   t

    14

    Visual obsessions (violent and horrific images),

    washing, checking, hoarding, specific phobias of

    spiders and blood. Past history of anorexia

    nervosa and bulimia nervosa.

    Initials of cases refer to the authors of the present article. All cases were assessed with the Y-BOCS. *Classified as employed because the person has an income.

    pt 15

  • 8/20/2019 Musical Obsessions

    58/61

    Page 57 of 60

       A  c  c  e   p   t  e  d

        M  a  n   u

      s  c  r   i   p   t

    15

    Table A3. Summary of treatments for previously published case studies of musical obsessions. 

    Case Treatment and Outcome

    1.  Ahuja (2001) Distraction and fluoxetine (40 mg/day).

    2. 

    Ahuja (2001) Fluvoxamine (200 mg/day), buspirone (40 mg/day).3.  Andrade & Rao

    (1997)

    Patient was offered pharmacotherapy but declined. Preferred to try to control the obsessions on his own.

    4.  Cameron &

    Wasielewski

    (1990)

    Clomipramine up to 250 mg/day for 14 weeks. Statistical and clinically significant improvement. Patient was able to return to school.

    Prior to receiving clomipramine the patient had failed to respond to other, unspecified medications.

    5.  Fontenelle

    (2008)

    Clomipramine up to 100 mg/day for 12 weeks. OCD was rated as very much improved according to both the CGI (score=1) and

    YBOCS (score=5). Given the very low scores it i s most likely that her musical obsession abated with treatment a lthough this is not

    explicitly stated by the author.

    6.  Gomibuchi et al.

    (2000) –  Case 1

    Diazepam (6mg/day) along with advice not to study too hard and not to attempt to suppress musical imagery. Five sessions in all.

    Reported being symptom free at 5-year followup.

    7.  Gomibuchi et al.

    (2000) –  Case 2

    Bromazepam (6mg/day) along with advice not to study too hard and not to attempt to suppress musical imagery. Three sessions in all.

    Musical obsession diminished within a week. Relapsed at 5-year followup but then improved after h


Recommended