+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

Date post: 11-Sep-2021
Category:
Upload: others
View: 2 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
109
Retrospective eses and Dissertations Iowa State University Capstones, eses and Dissertations 1995 Personality and interests of giſted adolescents: differences by gender and domain Rachel Harriet Heiss Iowa State University Follow this and additional works at: hps://lib.dr.iastate.edu/rtd Part of the Developmental Psychology Commons , and the Personality and Social Contexts Commons is Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Iowa State University Capstones, eses and Dissertations at Iowa State University Digital Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Retrospective eses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Iowa State University Digital Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Recommended Citation Heiss, Rachel Harriet, "Personality and interests of giſted adolescents: differences by gender and domain " (1995). Retrospective eses and Dissertations. 10907. hps://lib.dr.iastate.edu/rtd/10907
Transcript
Page 1: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

Retrospective Theses and Dissertations Iowa State University Capstones, Theses andDissertations

1995

Personality and interests of gifted adolescents:differences by gender and domainRachel Harriet HeissIowa State University

Follow this and additional works at: https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/rtd

Part of the Developmental Psychology Commons, and the Personality and Social ContextsCommons

This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Iowa State University Capstones, Theses and Dissertations at Iowa State UniversityDigital Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Retrospective Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Iowa State UniversityDigital Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected].

Recommended CitationHeiss, Rachel Harriet, "Personality and interests of gifted adolescents: differences by gender and domain " (1995). Retrospective Thesesand Dissertations. 10907.https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/rtd/10907

Page 2: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

INFORMATION TO USERS

This manuscript has been reproduced from the microfilm master. UMI films the text directly from the original or copy submitted. Thus, some thesis and dissertation copies are in typewriter face, while others may be from any type of computer printer.

The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs, print bleedthrough, substandard margins, and in^oper alignment can adversely affect reproduction.

In the unlikely event that the author did not send UMI a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion.

Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, charts) are reproduced by sectioning the original, beginning at the upper left-hand comer and

continuing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. Each original is also photographed in one exposure and is included in reduced form at the back of the book.

Photogrq)hs included in the original manuscript have been reproduced xerographically in this copy. Higher quality 6" x 9" black and white photographic prints are available for aity photographs or illustrations appearing in this cof^ for an additional charge. Contact UMI directly to order.

A Bell & Howell information Company 300 North Zeeb Road. Ann Arbor. Ml 48106-1346 USA

313/761-4700 800/521-0600

Page 3: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents
Page 4: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

Personality and interests of gifted adolescents:

Differences by gender and domain

by

Rachel Harriet Heiss

A Dissertation Submitted to the

Graduate Faculty in Partial Fulfillment of the

Requirements for the Degree of

DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY

Department: Psychology Major; Psychology

Approved;

In Charge of Major Wprk

For the Ma3or Department

For the Graduate College

Iowa State University Ames, Iowa

1995

Signature was redacted for privacy.

Signature was redacted for privacy.

Signature was redacted for privacy.

Signature was redacted for privacy.

Page 5: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

OMI Number: 9531743

OMI Microform 9531743

Copyright 1995, by OMI Company. All rights reserved.

This microform edition is protected against unauthorized

copying under Title 17, United States Code.

UMI 300 North Zeeb Road

Ann Arbor, MI 48103

Page 6: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

11

TABLE OF CONTENTS

PAGE

GENERAL INTRODUCTION 1

Statement of the Problem 1 Dissertation Organization 2

PERSONALITY AND INTERESTS OF GIFTED INDIVIDUALS: A LITERATURE REVIEW 4

INTRODUCTION 4

REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE 5

CONCLUSION 20

REFERENCES 22

PERSONALITY AND INTERESTS OF GIFTED ADOLESCENTS: DIFFERENCES BY GENDER AND DOMAIN 30

ABSTRACT 30

INTRODUCTION 30

METHOD 39

RESULTS 46

DISCUSSION 61

REFERENCES 78

GENERAL SUMMARY 91

ADDITIONAL REFERENCES 93

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 94

APPENDIX; DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS FOR THE SCALES 95 USED IN THIS STUDY

Page 7: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

1

GENERAL INTRODUCTION

Statement of the Problem

The personality structure of individuals with high

intelligence has been the subject of speculation throughout

history, fueled by observations that these people often differ

substantially from their peers in ways that intuitively appear

unrelated to their precocity. Myths and half-truths about

such individuals have proliferated, some of them persisting

today.

In spite of the development of more precise scientific

procedures and sophisticated statistical methodology, current

empirical literature concerning the nature and personalities

of highly-intelligent people can be described as inconsistent

and confusing in its findings.

A number of methodological problems contribute to these

inconsistencies. The definition of the construct intelligence

lacks consensus (Benbow & Minor, 1990), and its

operationalization is diverse, with some researchers using

achievement criteria and others using standardized

intelligence measures. Furthermore, cut-off IQ scores used to

define intellectual precocity range from 120 to 180, a

difference of four standard deviations. It is unreasonable to

expect such individuals to share personality traits related to

the commonality of high intelligence when the ability of these

subjects varies as much as four standard deviations and they

were selected using different types of measures.

Page 8: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

2

Furthermore, a number of current researchers emphasize

that intellectual giftedness is not a unitary construct, but

rather should be conceptualized as consisting of multiple

talents (Benbow & Minor, 1990). The research presented here

was designed to excimine the personality characteristics of

adolescents who are all highly talented, but in different

domains. Specifically, it examined the questions: Are

personality characteristics related to domains of talent?

And, if so, what characteristics are associated with what

domain? Are there interactions with gender?

One ideal of our current educational system is to help

all of our young people strive to reach their potential. Many

of our most talented youngsters fall far short of this goal.

Feelings of perfectionism, isolation, and alienation often

intrude, hindering both academic achievement and personal life

satisfaction. The potential findings of this research could

increase our understanding of the inner world of these

students and thus have implications for gifted education,

which strives to plan progreuns and interventions to help them

achieve.

Dissertation Organization

This dissertation has been developed according to an

alternate format and includes the following: (1) a general

introduction, (2) a literature review manuscript entitled

"Personality and Interests of Gifted Individuals: A Literature

Review," (3) a research study manuscript entitled "Personality

Page 9: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

3

and Interests of Gifted Adolescents: Differences by Gender and

Domain," with figures included after the references, (4) a

general summary of the entire dissertation, and (5) additional

references for the general introduction and the general

summary.

Page 10: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

4

PERSONALITY AND INTERESTS OF GIFTED INDIVIDUALS: A LITERATURE REVIEW

A paper prepared for submission to the Gifted Child Quarterly

Rachel Heiss and Ccunilla Perrson Benbow

INTRODUCTION

Interest, opinions, and speculation concerning the

characteristics of people who are highly intelligent have a

long and diverse history. Through the ages it has been

observed that individuals who are highly intelligent often

differ substantially from their peers. This phenomenon has

encouraged the proliferation of myths and half-truths

concerning the personalities of people who are highly

intelligent. For over six decades now, respected scholars

such as Lewis Terman, Leta Hollingworth, and others have been

publishing research that refutes many of the earlier

misconconceptions, but some of these myths persist today

(Haier & Solano, 1976).

The purpose of this paper is to review the research

literature as it relates to the personality characteristics of

people who are intellectually gifted. Special attention is

given to differences related to gender and domain of

giftedness. The nature of our intellectually-gifted

population is a significant issue: They are an important

resource, and many fail to even approach the achievement of

which they are capable. It is both humane and in our

Page 11: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

5

collective self-interest to pursue better understanding of

this population. Various methodological issues have resulted

in a body of literature that is confusing and inconsistent

about this important subject. They are addressed in the

literature review.

REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE

Historical Development

Throughout recorded history, individuals possessing

extremely high intellectual ability have been regarded as

being different from their peers. Sometimes they were thought

to be deities, at other times, demons (Grinder, 1985). In the

nineteenth century, precocity continued to be associated with

pathology (Lombroso, 1891), in spite of Galton's observations

to the contrary (Galton, 1969/1892). Research by Lewis T.

Terman (1925) and Leta S. Hollingworth (1942) refuted much of

the negative mythology, but negative stereotypes still have

persisted (Haier & Solano, 1976), and impressions of many

clinicians and educators are at variance with the bulk of

research findings.

Do High-Ability Students Differ from their Peers?

Terman (1925) and Hollingworth (1942) both argued that

highly-gifted students are superior to their peers in many

characteristics that are intuitively independent of

intelligence. Other researchers (Killian, 1983; Mayer,

Caruso, Zigler, and Dreyden, 1987; Smith, 1962) have found few

differences separating gifted students from their classmates.

Page 12: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

6

Furthermore, even research studies finding differences do not

necessarily agree about the characteristics differentiating

highly-intelligent students from non-gifted classmates. These

inconsistencies can be partially explained by the diverse

criteria used to define the population, including IQ's ranging

from 120 to 180 and above. In addition, many studies have not

considered the different domains of giftedness nor the fact

that gender may interact with domain. A closer exeunination of

these issues reveals that these are important considerations

that shed light on the personalities of these people.

Characteristics of the Gifted

Introversion

Introversion is a basic personality trait that has been

identified as Factor I of the Big-Five factor structure

identified by a number of leading personality theorists

(Digman, 1990; McCrae & Costa, 1985), and the

introversion/extroversion continuum is one of two basic

personality factors in Eysenck's theory (Eysenck & Eysenck,

1969). Even though Eysenck (1971) claimed that intelligence

is not related to introversion, a number of other researchers

have found that gifted children tend to be introverts

(Silverman, 1993a; Berndt, Kaiser, & van Aalst, 1982) and that

the likelihood of introversion increases with the IQ score

(Silverman, 1986).

Haier and Denheun (1976), using the Eysenck Personality

Inventory (EPI; Eysenck & Eysenck, 1964), found

Page 13: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

7

mathematically-gifted boys to be both more introverted and

more interpersonally effective than comparison groups. Using

Holland's Vocational Preference Inventory (Holland, 1975),

they also found mathematically-gifted students of both genders

to be predominantly Investigative, a finding that supports

introversion as an attribute of mathematically-gifted young

people. David L. Robinson (1985) examined the relationship

of ability to introversion by studying the relationship of

subtest profiles on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale

(WAIS; Wechsler, 1955) to the Extraversion (E) scale of the

Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ; Eysenck, 1975). He

found that introverts tend to do better on Verbal subtests,

while extroverts perform higher on Performance subtests. It

must be noted that the Arithmetic subtest is included under

"Verbal," and that the Performance subtests, which measure

visual perceptual and spatial abilities, are timed. It is

possible that the salient characteristic of Performance

subtests as related to introversion is the time component.

Isolation

A characteristic related to introversion is that of

isolation or, at least, a willingness to be alone. Several

authors have identified a willingness, or even preference, to

be alone as common among the intellectually gifted (Albert,

1978; McCurdy, 1957). Such isolation seems to be more likely

when the child is highly gifted (Hollingworth, 1942). For

example, Sheldon (1959) used the Rorschach inkblot test plus

Page 14: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

8

various sociometric studies, classroom observations, and

interviews to study the issue of isolation in students with IQ

scores of 170 or higher. He concluded that, although high

intelligence may contribute to isolation, it is not a

"sufficient cause" (p. 220) of isolation. He theorized that

it may result from dyneunics of feunily and school roles in the

lives of highly-intelligent students. Other researchers

theorize that isolation comes from a sense of feeling

"different" from their peers (Janos, Fung, & Robinson, 1985;

Janos & Robinson, 1985). Indeed, intelligent children are

different from their peers in both interests and vocabulary

(Hollingworth, 1942) and, thus, may have trouble finding

compatible friends (Greenlaw & Mcintosh, 1988).

Correspondingly, the peers of gifted children may also

perceive the gifted children as being different from

themselves and hence tend to exclude them (Janos, Fung, &

Robinson, 1985).

A sense of isolation may be exacerbated by a tendency

toward nonconformity fostered by a willingness to make their

own judgments (Brode, 1980) and also by their selection of

more solitary recreational activities, such as fishing,

walking (Roe, 1952), and reading (Hollingworth, 1942).

Isolation can even be viewed as a by-product of their love of

learning: Learning is often a solitary pursuit.

Page 15: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

9

Perfectionism

Perfectionism is another trait frequently identified as

characteristic of gifted children (Hollingworth, 1926; Kerr,

1991; Robinson & Noble, 1991). In fact, Whitmore (1980)

considered striving for perfection to be a key component of

giftedness. Silverman (1993a) offered a possible explanation

of this perfectionism by relating it to the good abstract

reasoning also characteristic of gifted individuals. She

reasoned that the gifted child is capable of visualizing or

conceptualizing perfection due to their extraordinary

reasoning abilities.

Need for Precision

A trait closely related to perfectionism is a need for

precision. Hollingworth (1927) recognized that gifted

children often have an extraordinary need for precision. In

fact, Silverman (1993a) used this need for precision to

explain the trait of argumentativeness. These children feel a

need to correct errors and may not realize that, to others,

the experience of being corrected is aversive. Kline and

Meckstroth (1985) identified the need for precision as

especially characteristic of the extremely gifted.

Self-sufficiencv

According to Warren and Heist (1960), self-sufficiency is

the trait most often found to be characteristic of the

intellectually gifted. This supports the observations of

Hildreth (1938), who said such a child "keeps himself busy by

Page 16: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

10

setting tasks for himself" (p. 301). It is also consistent

with the findings of Gottsdanker (1968)/ who found that gifted

women strongly value autonomy and non-authoritarianism.

Sense of Humor

Gifted children also appreciate humor (Hildreth, 1938).

Their delight in puns and word plays (Greenlaw & Mcintosh,

1988) suggests that their sense of humor is related to well-

developed verbal ability. It likely is also related to their

ability to grasp relationships quickly. Shade (1991)

conducted research on the reponse of gifted students to humor

and found them more responsive to verbal humor than their

peers from the general population.

Energetic

Terman (1925) and Miles (1954) described gifted

individuals as having high energy. Although they were

referring to physical energy, traits such as enthusiasm, as

described by Halpin, Payne, and Ellett (1975), and mental

vigor, as described by Carter (1958), suggest that the trait

of high energy is broader and includes the mental domain as

well. In fact, Hildreth (1938) commented on their

"inexhaustible supply of mental energy" (p. 301). The

overexcitabilities described by Dabrowski (1972) and

Piechowski (1991) and the excitability cited by Lovecky (1993)

support the pervasiveness of this characteristic.

Page 17: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

11

Curiosity and Need for Intellectual Challenge

Cox (1981) identified curiosity as "one of the most

pervasive characteristics" (p. 113) of intellectually gifted

children. Other researchers (Subotnik, Summers, Kassan, &

Wasser, 1993) have documented gifted children's need to have

their curiosity satisfied. Similarly, Hollingworth (1942)

commented that gifted children enjoy complicated games and

activities requiring intellectual skill, and Lovecky (1992)

commented on the need for mental stimulation as especially

characteristic of highly-intelligent individuals.

Extreme Levels of Precocity

Although degrees of mental retardation are recognized as

having important practical implications (Milgram, 1991),

degrees of precocity are not given much consideration. In

fact, extreme precocity is the least-studied aspect in the

area of intellectual giftedness (Feldman, 1979).

Leta Hollingworth (1942) recognized the "special

perplexities" (p. 253) of highly-precocious children and

recent research confirms that they are, indeed, different in

behavior, achievement, and values from their less-precocious

peers.

In a comparison of students scoring in the top 1/4 of the

top one percent on the Scholastic Aptitude Test (SAT) with

those scoring the lower 1/4 of the top one percent, Benbow

(1992) found that those in the top group achieved far more

than those in the lower group. In a different study, Benbow

Page 18: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

12

(1986) also found the physiological characteristics of left

handednessr allergies, and myopia to be correlated with

extreme precococity. Brody and Benbow (1986) concluded that

highly-gifted individuals fit in less well and are less

popular than their peers who are less-highly gifted.

Furthermore, Fox (1976) uncovered a striking relationship in

the degree of mathematical precocity of adolescent males with

the importance of theoretical values as measured on the

Allport, Vernon, and Lindzey's Study of Values (1970). In

addition, Kline and Meckstroth (1985) have identified need for

precision as especially descriptive of the highly gifted, and

Lovecky (1992) argued that the extremely-precocious

individuals have an extraordinarily high need for mental

stimulation. Feldman (1984) reported that Terman's female

subjects with IQs of 180 and above had much more interest in

careers than those with IQs of 150.

Evidence concerning the mental health of individuals who

are extremely precocious is mixed. Several recent studies

(Gallucci, 1988; Grossberg & Cornell, 1988) suggest that

extremely high ability children have no more psychopathology

than their less-gifted peers.

The bulk of the research, however, supports

Hollingworth's (1942) thesis that children with IQ scores

above 150 or 160 are especially vulnerable to isolation and

alienation (Janos & Robinson, 1985). A study by the New York

Page 19: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

13

University Counseling Center of more than 100 children with

IQ's of 170 or higher revealed that these children

characteristically had trouble finding understanding

companions and experienced loneliness (Wall, 1960). Lewis

(1943) compared a group of gifted students with another group

he called very gifted and found the very gifted more likely to

be maladjusted. In a study of isolation in children with IQs

of 170 and above, Sheldon (1959) concluded that high

intelligence may contribute to isolation but is not a

"sufficient cause" (p. 220) of it. He also found the very

gifted to display deep feelings of inadequacy and to lack

self-confidence.

Gender Differences

According to Fox (1977), studies of the gifted tend to

ignore important gender differences. Silverman (1993b) argued

that this lack of attention to gender differences is

especially salient because females pay a higher social price

for their high ability. Females with high grade point

averages are more depressed, have more psychosomatic symptoms

and have lower self-esteem. Silverman also found a gender

bias in that females are more likely to be labeled as

overachievers and bossy.

Researchers have reported for a long time that

personality characteristics and interests of gifted males and

females tend to be more similar to each other than they are to

their peers of the same gender in the general population

Page 20: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

14

(Lubinski & Humphreys, 1990; Terman & Oden, 1947; Warren &

Heist, 1960). In fact, using the California Personality

Inventory (Gough, 1969), Haier and Denham (1976) found the

personality profiles of mathematically-gifted males and

females to be similar on all but the femininity measure.

There is some difference of opinion on which sex differs

more from gender norms. Werner and Bachtold (1969) argued

that gifted males differ more. Haier and Denham (1976) found

mathematically-gifted females to be much more unconventional

and nonconforming when compared to female comparison groups

than are males when compared to male comparison groups; and

Gottsdanker (1968) found gifted females to diverge more from

average females than the gifted males diverged from average

males. Gifted women were especially high in autonomy or

nonauthoritarianism, in interest in theoretical problems, and

in their pursuit of self-initiated intellectual endeavors.

Pox, Pasternack, and Peiser (1976) found gifted females to be

interested in career areas traditionally regarded as masculine

as well as those considered to be feminine. Males, however,

did not evidence reciprocal interest in careers regarded as

feminine.

Recent literature consistently reports that gifted males

and females differ somewhat in interests and values.

Gottsdanker (1968) found gifted males to score higher on

impulsivity and religious liberalism. Fox, Pasternack, and

Peiser (1976) found mathematically-gifted girls more

Page 21: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

15

interested in the social and artistic areas. In addition,

males demonstrate more variability in personality

characteristics (Callahan, 1979; Helson, 1968; Werner &

Bachtold/ 1969).

Much of the recent literature looking at gender

differences in the gifted population has limited the focus to

one of the principal areas of ability, often mathematical or

verbal precocity.

Gender Differences among the Mathematically Precocious

Lubinski and Benbow (1992) documented both cognitive and

noncognitive gender differences in the data collected by the

Study of Mathematically Precocious Youth (SMPY). The most

startling difference was the disproportionate ratio of males

to females at the extremely high levels of mathematical

reasoning: Of over a million seventh-graders taking the SAT-

Mathematics (SAT-M) test as part of a talent search, the ratio

of males to females scoring 700 or higher, is 13/1 (Benbow,

1988). SMPY also discovered that in the population of

mathematically-gifted students, there are significant gender

differences in values, theoretical values being more

characteristic of males, and social values being more

characteristic of females.

According to Lubinski, Benbow, and Sanders (1993), gifted

female adolescents are less differentiated than gifted male

adolescents in their vocational preferences and interests.

For excunple, on Holland's Hexagon, interests of females are

Page 22: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

16

more evenly distributed on the Investigative, Social, and

Artistic sectors, while the interests of the males are

strongly Investigative, with the Realistic dimension a distant

second choice. A different study (Fox & Denheun, 1974) found

that a majority of mathematically-gifted boys preferred the

Investigative occupations and scored much lower on all the

other categories. Mathematically-gifted girls, however, were

less differentiated, scoring highest on the Artistic category.

They also scored high on Investigative and Social, but

extremely low on Enterprising, Conventional, and Realistic.

Their interest in Social occupations was remarkably different;

Social occupations were selected by almost one-fourth of the

females but by less than five percent of the males. Neither

gender showed interest in Conventional occupations.

Gender Differences of the Verbally Precocious

McGinn (1976) conducted a study that addressed gender

differences in a population of adolescents in which all the

subjects were verbally gifted, using the Study of Values (SOV;

Allport, Vernon, & Lindzey, 1970), the California

Psychological Inventory (Gough, 1969), and the Self-Directed

Search (Holland, 1970). Both verbally-gifted girls and

verbally-gifted boys were described as independent,

imaginative, original, spontaneous, analytic and insightful,

but the girls were found to be more interested in writing,

cultural activities, and arts and crafts, as well as more

interpersonally understanding than the boys. The boys, on the

Page 23: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

17

other hand, demonstrated more interest in science and in

mechanical and electronic objects than the girls. In

comparing values, the boys scored higher than the girls on

Theoretical, Economic, and Political scales of the SOV, while

the girls scored higher than the boys on the Aesthetic,

Social, and Religious scales. Both scored relatively high on

Holland's Investigative scale, suggesting that both genders

are analytical, curious, introspective, and precise. The

girls, however, were even higher on the Artistic scale,

supporting the earlier-mentioned finding that they are more

interested in cultural and artistic activities than the boys.

A comparison, by gender, of verbally-gifted adolescents

with norms of non-gifted adolescents also shows some

differences (McGinn, 1976). Verbally-gifted boys were found

to be less pragmatic and more considerate of others than their

male peers. Similarly, verbally-gifted girls were more

intellectual and competitive, but less pragmatic or

religiously-oriented than female peers.

Domains of Talent

There is considerable evidence for personality

differences according to areas of giftedness (Andreason, 1978;

Elton & Rose, 1967; Roe, 1952). Some studies have approached

this question by studying gifted individuals with significant

discrepancies in their abilities. Using both objective and

projective measures, McCarthy (1975) found primarily

quantitative subjects to demonstrate more objectivity and

Page 24: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

18

conventionality, while the primarily verbal subjects had less

respect for authority.

Ferguson and Maccoby (1966) studied personality

correlates with children manifesting discrepancies between

their verbal, spatial, and mathematical abilities. An

interesting conclusion was that aggressiveness in males was

positively related to mathematical ability and negatively

related to verbal ability. However, they went on to

differentiate between types of aggressiveness and suggested

that mathematical ability is related to more "appropriate" or

productive type of aggressiveness, while the high-verbal males

reported a more antisocial aggression related to hostility.

In addition, they concluded that high-mathematical children

were more gregarious and socially secure than the high-verbal

children.

A study by Sanders, Mefferd, and Bown (1960) used both

psychological and physiological measures and divided male

subjects into three groups: high verbal and high

quantitative, high verbal and low quantitative, and low verbal

and high quantitative. Significant differences in personality

and metabolic characteristics as well as scholastic

achievement were found among the three groups.

The high verbal-low quantitative group scored especially

high on autonomy and low on endurance and were independent,

demonstrating little need for affiliation or conformity.

Paradoxically, they did demonstrate a need to be the center of

Page 25: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

19

attention. In making decisions, they tended to rely on their

ovm feelings and perceptions instead of consulting with

others. This high verbal, low quantitative group differed the

most from the other two groups. Subjects high in both verbal

and quantitative abilities scored high on dominance. They

were also decisive, demonstrating only a moderate eunount of

autonomy or affiliation, and avoided unconventional responses.

Interpersonal competence appeared relatively unimportant to

them. The high quantitative, low verbal subjects were

introspective, systematic, and perseverent, more apt to obey

authority and value affiliation.

In a study of women at Sarah Lawrence, Munroe (1946)

found women who had high quantitative abiity to be more

literal and women with high linguistic ability to be more

subjective in their construction of reality. In a comparison

of women who were primarily quantitative with those who were

primarily linguistic in ability, Altus (1952) found the

quantitative women to be more prim, conventional, iimnature,

and anxious. In a similar study using college males, Altus

(1958) found the primarily linguistic males to be more

sophisticated. On the other hand, the primarily quantitative

males had scores on the Masculine/Feminine scale of the

Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) that

indicated more masculine attitudes. Studies of Munroe (1946)

and Altus, (1952, 1958) indicated that although subjects with

varying intellectual abilities differ in personality, personal

Page 26: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

20

adjustment is not related to their mix of verbal and nonverbal

abilities.

A study by Jensen (1994) which used the MMPI (Hathaway &

McKinley, 1943) to compare mathematically and verbally-gifted

adolescents (and a normative control group that was comparable

in SES) underscores the importance of both gender and domain

of talent. Her findings suggested that^ although the

psychological profiles of all the groups were within normal

limits, the verbally-gifted females had higher levels of

depression than the comparison groups and more paranoia than

the female comparison groups. To a lesser degree,

mathematically-gifted males also demonstrated more depression

than comparison groups.

CONCLUSION

This review of the literature has gathered information

about what is now known about the nature of intellectually-

gifted individuals: Intellectually-gifted people have a

strong tendency to be introverted (Silverman, 1993a; Berndt,

Kaiser, & van Aalst, 1982), and the likelihood of introversion

increases as IQ scores go up (Silverman, 1986). Gifted

individuals often experience a sense of isolation (Albert,

1978; McCurdy, 1957), and the extremely precocious are

especially vulnerable to alienation and loneliness

(Hollingworth, 1942) related to their difficulty in finding

peers. Characteristics, such as perfectionism (Hollingworth,

1926), self-suffiency (Warren and Heist, 1960), and curiosity

Page 27: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

21

(Cox, 1981), are commonly recognized, but the intensity with

which these individuals experience their world is easily

overlooked and may be even more important in terms of really

understanding them. Dabrowski (1972) and Piechowski (1991)

have used the word "overexcitabilities" to describe this

intensity, which can extend to all areas of their lives.

Highly-intelligent people also demonstrate significant

differences related to gender and the domain of their talent.

Gender differences are especially strong in values and

interests: The males are highly interested in theoretical and

investigative issues. Females are less differentiated in

their interests and are much more interested in social and

cultural activities than the males (Lubinski, Benbow, &

Sanders, 1993). When comparing the domains of mathematical

and verbal ability, the mathematically-talented are

predictably more conventional and conforming than the

verbally-talented (Sanders, Mefferd, & Bown, 1960).

Further study is needed, using more homogeneous groupings

of this population, with additional attention given to gender

and domains of giftedness as well as to degrees or levels of

precocity. More precise information garnered from such

research would be invaluable in increasing understanding of

such students and have implications for gifted education,

which strives to help such children reach their goals.

Page 28: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

22

REFERENCES

Albert, R. S. (1978). Observations and suggestions regarding giftedness, familial influence, and the achievement of eminence. Gifted Child Quarterly. 22. 201-211.

Allport, G. W., Vernon, P. E., & Lindzey, G. (1970). Manual for the studv of values; A scale for measuring the dominant interests in personality. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Altus, W. D. (1952). Personality correlates of Q-L variability on the ACE. Journal of Consulting Psychology. 16., 284-291.

Altus, VI. D. (1958). Q-L variability, MMPI responses, and college males. Journal of Consulting Psychology. 22., 367-371.

Andreasen, N. C. (1978). Creativity and psychiatric illness. Psychiatric annals. 8(3), 23-35.

Benbow, C. P. (1986). Physiological correlates of extreme intellectual precocity. Neuropsvchologia. 24., 719-725.

Benbow, C. P. (1988). Sex differences in mathematical reasoning ability among the intellectually talented: Their characterization, consequences, and possible explanations. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 11. 169-232.

Benbow, C. P. (1992). Academic achievement in mathematics and science of students between ages 13 and 23: Are there differences among students in the top one percent of mathematically ability? Journal of Educational Psychology. 84. 51-61.

Berndt, D. J., Kaiser, C. F., & van Aalst, F. (1982). Depression and self-actualization in gifted adolescents. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 38. 142-150.

Brode, D. S. (1980). Group dynamics with gifted adolescents. G/C/T. 15, 60-62.

Brody, L. E., & Benbow, C. P. (1986). Social and emotional adjustment of adolescents extremely talented in verbal or mathematical reasoning. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 15. 1-18.

Page 29: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

23

Callahan, C. M. (1979). The gifted and talented woman. In H. Passow & K. J. Rehage (Eds.), The gifted and the talented; Their education and development (pp. 401-423). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Carter, T. (1958). The play problems of gifted children. School Sociology. 86. 224-225.

Cox, R. L. (1981). Personal, physical, and family traits of gifted children. In B. S. Miller & M. Price (Eds.), The gifted child, the feunilv and the community (pp. 107-113). New York: Walker & Company.

Dabrowski, K. (1972). Psvchoneurosis is not an illness. London; Gryf.

Digman, J. M. (1990). Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor model. In M. R. Rosenzweig & L. W. Porter (Eds.), Annual review of psychology (Vol. 41, pp. 417-440). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.

Elton, C. F., & Rose, H. A. (1967). Traditional sex attitudes and discrepant ability measures in college women. Journal of Counseling Psychology. JL4, 538-543.

Eysenck, H. J. (1971). Relation between intelligence and personality. Perceptual Motor SKills. 32. 637-638.

Eysenck, H. J. (1975). Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire. London: Hodder & Stoughton.

Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1964). The Eysenck Personality Inventory. San Diego: Educational and Testing Service.

Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1969). Personality structure and measurement. San Diego: Robert Knapp.

Feldman, D. (1979). The mysterious case of extreme giftedness. In H. Passow & K. J. Rehage (Eds.), The gifted and the talented: Their education and development (pp. 335-351). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Feldman, D. H. (1984). A follow-up study of subjects who scored above 180 IQ in Terman's "Genetic Studies of Genius." Exceptional Children. 50, 518-523.

Ferguson, L. R., & Maccoby, E. E. (1966). Interpersonal correlates of differential abilities. Child Development. 32, 549-571.

Page 30: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

24

Fox, L. H. (1976). The values of gifted youth. In D. P. Keating (Ed.), Intellectual talent; Research and development (pp. 273-284). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Fox, L. H. (1977). Sex differences: Implications for program planning for the academically gifted. In J. C. Stanley, S. C. George, and C. H. Solano (Eds.), The gifted and the creative: A fiftv-vear perspective (pp. 113-138). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Unviersity Press.

Fox, L. H., & Denham, S. A. (1974). Values and career interests of mathematically and scientifically precocious youth. In J. C. Stanley, K. P. Keating, & L. H. Fox (Eds.), Mathematical talent: Discovery, description and development (pp. 140-175). Baltimore; Johns Hopkins University Press.

Fox, L. H., Pasternak, S. R., & Peiser, N. L. (1976). Career-related interests of adolescent boys and girls. In D. P. Keating (Ed.), Intellectual talent: Research and development (pp. 242-261). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Gallucci, N. T. (1988). Emotional adjustment of gifted children. Gifted Child Quarterly. 32, 273-276.

Galton, F. (1892). Hereditary genius. London: Julian Friedman. (original work published 1869).

Gottsdanker, J. S. (1968). Intellectual interest patterns of gifted college students. Educational and Psychological Measurement. 28., 361-366.

Gough, H. G. (1969). Manual for the California Psychological Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Greenlaw, M. J., & Mcintosh, M. E. (1988). Educating the gifted; A source book. Chicago: American Library Association.

Grinder, R. E. (1985). The gifted in our midst: By their divine deeds, neuroses, and mental test scores we have known them. In F. D. Horowitz & M. O'Brien (Eds.), The gifted and talented: Developmental perspectives (pp. 5-35). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Grossberg, I. N., & Cornell, D. G. (1988). Relationship between personality adjustment and high intelligence: Terman versus Hollingworth. Exceptional Children. 55. 266-272.

Page 31: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

25

Haier, R. J., & Denham, S. A. (1976). A summa^ profile of the nonintellectual correlates of mathematical precocity in boys and girls. In D. P. Keating (Ed.), Intellectual talent; Research and development (pp. 225-241). Baltimore; Johns Hopkins University Press.

Haier, R. J., & Solano, C. H. (1976). Educators' stereotypes of mathematically gifted boys. In D. P. Keating (Ed.), Intellectual talent; Research and development (pp. 215-222). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Halpin, G., Payne, D., & Ellett, C. (1975)^ Life history antecedents of current personality traits of gifted adolescents. Measurement and Evaluation in Guidance. 8(1), 29-36.

Hathaway, S. R., & McKinley, J. C. (1943). The Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (rev. ed.). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

Helson, R. (1968). Generality of sex differences in creative style. Journal of Genetic Psychology. 53. 287-311.

Hildreth, G. (1938). Characteristics of young gifted children. Journal of Genetic Psychology. 53. 287-311.

Holland, J. L, (1970). The Self-Directed Search. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Holland, J. L. (1975). Manual for the Vocational Preference Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Hollingworth, L. S. (1926). Gifted children; Their nature and nurture. New York: Macmillan.

Hollingworth, L. S. (1927). Who are gifted children? Child Study. 5(2), 3-5.

Hollingworth, L. S. (1942). Children above 180 10 Stanford Binet; Origin and development. Yonkers, NY: World Book.

Janos, P. M., & Robinson, N. M. (1985). Psychosocial development in intellectually gifted children. In F. D. Horowitz & M. O'Brien (Eds.), The gifted and talented; Developmental perspectives (pp. 149-195). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Page 32: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

26

Janos, P. M. / Fung, B. C., & Robinson, H. M. (1985). Self-concept, self-esteem, and peer relations among gifted children who feel "different." Gifted Child Quarterly. 29, 78-82.

Jensen, M. B. (1994). Psychological well-being of intellectually precocious youth and peers at commensurate levels of socioeconomic status. Unpublished master's thesis, Iowa State University, Ames, lA.

Kerr, B. A. (1991). A handbook for counseling the gifted and talented. Alexandria, VA: American Counseling Association.

Killian, J. (1983). Personality characteristics of intellectually gifted secondary students. Roeper Review. 5, 39-42.

Kline, B. E., & Meckstroth, E. A. (1985). Understanding and encouraging the exceptionally gifted. Roeper Review. 8, 24-30.

Lewis W. D. (1943). Some characteristics of very superior children. Journal of Genetic Psvcholoqv. 62. 301-309.

Lombroso, C. (1891). The man of genius. London: Scott.

Lovecky, D. V. (1992). The exceptionally gifted child. Understanding our Gifted. ̂ (3), 7-9.

Lovecky, D. V. (1993). The quest for meaning; Counseling issues with gifted children and adolescents. In L. K. Silvejnnan (Ed.), Counseling the gifted and talented (pp. 29-50). Denver; Love Publishing.

Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (1992). Gender differences in abilities and preferences among the gifted; Implications for the math-science pipeline. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 1, 61-66.

Lubinski, D., Benbow, C. P., & Sanders, C. E. (1993). Reconceptualizing gender differences in achievement among the gifted. In K. A. Heller, F. J. Monks, & A. H. Passow (Eds.), International handbood of research and development of qiftedness and talent (pp. 693-707). London: Pergcunon Press.

Lubinski, D., & Humphreys, L. G. (1990). A broadly based analysis of mathematical giftedness. Intelligence. 14. 327-355.

Page 33: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

27

Mayer, J. D., Caruso, 0. R., Zigler, E . , & Dreyden, J. I. (1987). Intelligence and intelligence-related personality traits. Intelligence. 13, 119-133.

McCarthy, S. V. (1975). Differential V-Q ability; Twenty years later. Review of Educational Research, 45. 263-2 8 2 .

McCurdy, H. G. (1957). The childhood pattern of genius. Journal of Elisha Mitchell Science Societv. 73. 448-462.

McGinn, P. V. (1976). Verbally gifted youth; Selection and description. In D. P. Keating (Ed.), Intellectual talent; Research and development (pp. 160-182). Baltimore; Johns Hopkins University Press.

McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1985). Validation of the five-factor model of personality across instruments and observers. Journal of Personalitv and Social Psvcholoav. 52, 81-90.

Miles, C. C. (1954). Gifted children. In L. Carmichael (Ed.), Manual of child psvcholoav (pp. 984-1008). New York; Wiley.

Milgram, R. M. (1991). Counseling gifted and talented children and youth: Who, where, what, how? In R. M. Milgrcun (Ed.), Counseling gifted and talented children (pp. 7-21). Norwood, NJ; Ablex publishing.

Munroe, R. L. (1946). Rorschach findings on college students showing different constellations of subscores on the ACE. Journal of Consulting Psvcholoav. 10. 301-316.

Piechowski, M. M. (1991). Emotional development and emotional giftedness. In N. Colangelo & G. A. Davis (Eds.), Handbook of gifted education, (pp. 285-307). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

Robinson, D. L. (1985). How personality relates to intelligence test performance: Implications for a theory of intelligence, ageing research and personality assessment. Personalitv and Individual Differences. 6, 203-216.

Robinson, N. M., & Noble, K. D. (1991). Social-emotional development and adjustment of gifted children. In M. C. Wang, M. C. Reynolds, & H. J. Walberg (Eds.), Handbook of special education. Research and practice; Vol. 4. Emerging programs (pp 57-76). New York: Pergamon.

Page 34: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

28

Roe, A. (1952). A psychologist examines 64 eminent scientists. Scientific American. 187. 21-25.

Sanders, E. M., Mefferd, R. B,, Jr., & Sown, 0. H. (1960). Verbal-quantitative ability and certain personality and metabolic characteristics of male college students. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 20. 491-503.

Shade, R. (1991). Verbal humor in gifted students and students in the general population: A comparison of spontaneous mirth and comprehension. Journal for the Education of the Gifted. 14. 134-150.

Sheldon, P. M. (1959). Isolation as a characteristic of highly gifted children. Journal of Educational Sociology. 32. 215-222.

Silyerman, L. K. (1986). Parenting young gifted children. Journal of Children in Contemporary Society. 18. 73-87.

Silverman, L. K. (1993a). A developmental model for counseling the gifted. In L. K. Silverman (Ed.), Counseling the gifted and talented (pp. 51-78). Denver: Love Publishing.

Silverman, L. K. (1993b). Social development, leadership, and gender issues. In L. K. Silverman (Ed.), Counseling the gifted and talented (pp. 291-327). Denver: Love Publishing.

Smith, D. C. (1962). Personal and social adjustment of gifted adolescents. Research Monograph No. 4. Washington DC: National Education Association, Council for Exceptional Children.

Subotnik, R., Summers, E., Kassan, L., & Wasser, A. (1993). Genius revisited; High 10 children grown up. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing.

Terman, L. M. (1925). Genetic studies of genius; Mental and physical traits of a thousand gifted children. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Terman, L. M., & Oden, M. H. (1947). Genetic studies of genius; Vol. 4. The gifted child grows up; Twenty-five year follow-up of a superior group. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Wall, W. D. (1960). Highly intelligent children: Part I— T h e p s y c h o l o g y o f t h e g i f t e d . E d u c a t i o n a l R e s e a r c h . 2 , 101-110.

Page 35: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

29

Warren, J. R., & Heist, P. A. (I960). Personality attributes of gifted college students. Science. 132, 330-337.

Werner, E. E., & Bachtold, L. M. (1969). Personality factors of gifted boys and girls in middle childhood and adolescence. Psychology in the schools. 6, 177-182.

Wechsler, D. (1955). Manual for Wechaler Adult Intelligence Scale. New York: Psychological Corporation.

Whitmore, J. R. (1980). Giftedness. conflict, and underachievement. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Page 36: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

30

PERSONALITY AND INTERESTS OF GIFTED ADOLESCENTS: DIFFERENCES BY GENDER AND DOMAIN

A paper prepared for submission to the Journal of Counseling Psychology

Rachel Heiss and Ccunilla Persson Benbow

ABSTRACT

Personality characteristics of intellectually gifted

adolescents were examined to determine whether this population

substantially differs from the general population, differs by

gender, and manifests characteristics related to talent

domain. The Adjective Check List, Study of Values,

Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire, and Strong

Interest Inventory were used to make comparisons. As a group

the subjects demonstrated a highly-developed thirst for

knowledge. They had a higher than average sense of

alienation, but also demonstrated high self-confidence.

Intellectually-gifted girls were more interested in people and

aesthetic pursuits than the boys and also demonstrated more

interest in religion. Verbally-gifted adolescents were less

sociable, but more inclined to push the limits than their

mathematically-gifted peers.

INTRODUCTION

Throughout history intellectual precocity has been

regarded with interest, but it has not necessarily been valued

by society (Grinder, 1985). Myths and half-truths about the

nature of highly-intelligent individuals persist today (Haier

Page 37: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

31

& Solano, 1976) in spite of extensive research that has

refuted many of them. Considering the potential for

contribution to our society from such individuals, it is

important both to understand and to help them use their

abilities to benefit both themselves and others. This study

is an attempt to gather more precise information about the

characteristics of this population by carefully defining the

groups by domain and by separating them by gender and then

comparing the groups to see how they are similar and how they

are different from each other. When gifted individuals are

lumped in more heterogeneous groups for comparison to people

in the general population, differences related to domain and

gender are often masked and results become difficult to

interpret but also vary from study to study.

Many current researchers believe that intellectual

giftedness should not be conceptualized as a unitary

construct, but rather as consisting of multiple talents

(Benbow & Minor, 1990). Indeed, when intellectual talent has

been studied specifically by domains, it has been discovered

that mathematical and verbal precocity are related to

different combinations of cognitive abilities (Benbow & Minor,

1990; Dark & Benbow, 1990), different personality traits

(Benbow & Minor, 1990; Payne, Halpin, & Ellet, 1973), and even

different political philosophies (Braungart, 1975) and values

(Fox, 1976). Gifted males and females also differ (Fox, 1977;

Lubinski & Benbow, 1992; Lubinski, Benbow & Sanders, 1993).

Page 38: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

32

In addition to providing salient information about how

the different types of giftedness are related to personality,

this research sheds light on the inconsistencies that abound

in the research literature because of the lack of consensual

definition of giftedness (Benbow & Minor, 1990), the diverse

ways in which the construct of intelligence has been

operationalized, and insufficient attention often given to

gender issues (Fox, 1977). First, a sunanary of the existing

knowledge is provided.

Characteristics of the Gifted

Although several researchers have not found high-ability

individuals to differ significantly from their peers in

personality characteristics that are intuitively independent

of intelligence (see Killian, 1983; Mayer, Caruso, Zigler, &

Dreyden, 1987; Smith, 1962), many other researchers have found

significant differences, some of which are summarized below.

Characteristics descriptive of the intellectually-gifted

population in general are discussed first, followed by

information about personality as it relates to gender and

domain of talent.

Introversion

Gifted children tend to be introverts (Silverman, 1993;

Berndt, Kaiser, & van Aalst, 1982). Furthermore, the

likelihood of introversion increases as the IQ score goes up

(Silverman, 1986). This does not mean that they necessarily

have poor social skills, but rather that they tend to be

Page 39: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

33

reserved, preferring intellectual activities over social ones.

In fact, in a comparison of mathematically-gifted boys with a

normative group, Haier and Denham (1976) found mathematically

gifted boys to prefer intellectual pursuits; at the same time,

however, they also tended to be more socially effective in

their relationships with others. Haier and Denham also found

both mathematically-gifted boys and girls to be primarily

Investigative. That is, they want to understand the physical

world and solve abstract problems, and they often like to work

alone (Holland, 1975).

Isolation

A characteristic related to introversion is that of

isolation, or at least a willingness to be alone. Several

authors have identified a willingness, or even preference, to

be alone as common among the intellectually gifted (Albert,

1978; McCurdy, 1957). Hollingworth (1942) found isolation to

be even more likely when the child is highly gifted. Some

gifted children perceive themselves as "different" from their

peers and have lower self-esteem (Janos, Fung, & Robinson,

1985). Their unusual interests and vocabulary set them apart

(Hollingworth, 1942), making it difficult for them to find

compatible friends (Greenlaw & Mcintosh, 1988). This sense of

isolation may be exacerbated by a tendency toward

nonconformity fostered by a desire to make their own judgments

(Brode, 1980) and by their choice of more solitary

recreational activities (Hollingworth, 1942; Roe, 1952).

Page 40: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

34

Perfectionism

Perfectionism is frequently identified as characteristic

of gifted children (Hollingworth, 1926; Kerr, 1991; Whitmore,

1980). Silverman (1993) relates it to their good abstract

reasoning—that they can conceptualize perfection because of

their extraordinary reasoning abilities.

Need for Precision

Need for precision, a trait closely related to

perfectionism, is especially characteristic of the extremely

gifted (Kline & Meckstroth, 1985). Silverman (1993) uses this

trait to explain the argumentativeness of gifted children.

They feel a need to correct errors and may not realize that,

for others, the experience of being corrected can be aversive.

Self-Sufficiencv

Self-sufficiency is very characteristic of the

intellectually gifted (Hildreth, 1938; Warren & Heist, 1960).

Gottsdanker (1968) found gifted women to strongly value

autonomy and non-authoritarianism.

Sense of Humor

Gifted children appreciate humor (Hildreth, 1938),

especially verbal hvunor (Shade, 1991). Their delight in puns

and word plays (Greenlaw & Mcintosh, 1988), as well as other

verbal humor, suggests it is related to their well-developed

verbal ability and their ability to grasp relationships

easily.

Page 41: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

35

Energetic

Terman (1925) and Miles (1954) described highly-

intelligent individuals as having abundant energy. The

enthusiasm mentioned by Halpin, Payne, and Ellett (1975),

mental vigor cited by Carter (1958), and "inexhaustible supply

of mental energy" described by Hildreth (p. 301, 1938) suggest

that this high energy is broad in scope and includes the

mental domain as well.

The overexcitabilities noted by Dabrowski (1972) and

Piechowski (1991), as well as the excitability cited by

Lovecky (1993), further broaden the scope of this trait in

that they describe the intensity of thoughts and emotions

often experienced by highly-gifted people.

Curiosity and Need for Intellectual Challenge

Curiosity and the need for intellectual stimulation are

highly characteristic of very intelligent individuals (Cox,

1981; Lovecky, 1992; Subotnik, Summers, Kassan, & Wasser,

1993), leading them to enjoy complicated games and activities

requiring intellectual skill (Hollingworth, 1942).

Gender Differences

Recent literature that considers the issue of gender and

how it relates to giftedness indicates that gifted males and

females differ somewhat in interests and values.

Gender Differences of the Mathematically Precocious

Lubinski and Benbow (1992) have documented the gender

differences discovered by the Study of Mathematically

Page 42: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

36

Precocious Youth (SMPY), which began over 20 years ago. SMPY

discovered significant gender differences in values, with

theoretical values preferred by males and social values more

characteristic of females.

Lubinski, Benbow, and Sanders (1993) found gifted females

to be less differentiated than gifted males. On Holland's

Hexagon, interests of the girls are more evenly distributed on

the Investigative, Social and Artistic sectors, while the

interests of boys are strongly Investigative, with Realistic

as a distant second choice.

Fox and Denham (1974) also found significant differences

between mathematically-gifted boys and girls, with the boys

strongly preferring Investigative occupations over all others.

Girls had a much different pattern, scoring highest on

Artistic, but also scoring high on Investigative and Social.

The most striking difference was in Social occupations,

selected by almost one-fourth of the females but by less than

five percent of the males.

Gender Differences eunona the Verballv Precocious

McGinn (1976) found verbally-talented girls to be more

imaginative and more interested in writing, arts and crafts

and cultural activities than such boys. Verbally gifted boys,

on the other hand, demonstrated more interest in science,

mechanics, and electronics. On the Study of Values, (Allport,

Vernon & Lindzey, 1970) verbally-gifted boys scored highest on

Theoretical and Political and lowest on Religious. The girls

Page 43: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

37

scored higher on Social and Aesthetic and lowest on Economic.

On the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (Myers, 1962), both males

and females were seen as insightful and intellectually

curious. Males, however, were seen as more analytical and

females as more interpersonally understanding. Using the

Self-Directed Search (Holland, 1970), both verbally-gifted

males and females scored high on Investigative, but the girls

were even higher on Artistic.

Domains of Talent

McCarthy (1975), who exeimined personality indices of

subjects with discrepancies in their verbal and math

abilities, concluded that the primarily-quantitative subjects

were more objective and conventional and that the primarily-

verbal subjects had less respect for authority. In a similar

study exeunining personality correlates of children with

discrepancies between verbal, spatial, and mathematical

abilities, Ferguson and Maccoby (1966) concluded that

aggressiveness in males was positively related to mathematical

ability and negatively related to verbal ability. They went

on to differentiate between types of aggressiveness, however,

and suggested that mathematical ability is related to a more

appropriate or productive type of aggressiveness, while the

high-verbal males manifested a more antisocial aggression

related to hostility. They also found high-mathematical

children to be more gregarious and socially secure than the

high-verbal children.

Page 44: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

38

Sandersr Mefferd, and Bown (I960) divided their male

subjects into three groups: high verbal-high quantitative,

high verbal-low quantitative, and low verbal-high

quantitative. The high verbal-low quantitative group differed

most from the other two groups, showing little need for

affiliation or conformity, but wanting to be at the center of

attention. The high verbal-high quantitative group scored

high on dominance. They showed only moderate autonomy and

affiliation, but avoided unconventional responses. Low

verbal-high quantitative subjects were introspective,

systematic, and perseverent, and more apt to obey authority

and value affiliation.

Heretofore, the bulk of the research on intellectual

precocity has studied relatively heterogeneous groups, paying

little attention to domains of giftedness and often ignoring

gender, with the result that differences between groups are

often masked. Given the possible interactions between

characteristics, domain, and gender, one might ask: Are

characteristics of intellectually-gifted people related to the

domain of giftedness? Does the level of giftedness matter?

Do people with discrepancies in their abilities have

predictable traits? How does gender interact with other

factors in gifted individuals? These questions served as the

focus of this study.

This study is unique in several other ways. Instruments

used to make comparisons included well-known and

Page 45: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

39

psychometrically sound measures, like the Strong Interest

Inventory (Harmon, Hansen/ Borgen & Heunmer, 1994), Study of

Values (Allport, Vernon, & Lindsey, 1970), and the Adjective

Check List (Gough & Heilbrun, 1970), and the more modern

Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire (Tellegen & Waller,

1982). Hence, assessments were made using state-of-the-art

instrumentation. Additionally, although all the subjects have

intellectual gifts, individuals are differentiated by domain

of talent and then compared. In addition to attending to

levels of giftedness, groups are also defined by the type of

talent most highly manifested within each individual to see

whether intrapersonal discrepancies in domain of giftedness

are related to personality. Such comparisons using

homogeneous groups further our understanding by refining

knowledge that has been masked when more diverse groups of

intelligent individuals are compared with the general

population.

METHOD

Subjects

This study utilized information collected from students

who attended one of the progreuns designed for academically

gifted youth at Iowa State University during the summers of

1990, 1991, 1992, 1993, or 1994. These programs included CY-

TAG, Governor's Institute, and Explorations 1.

CY-TAG. CY-TAG is a suiraner residential program for

highly gifted youth, where students enroll in an academic

Page 46: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

40

class that meets for six hours each day for three weeks and is

taught at the college freshman level. The average class size

is 20 students. Students were eligible for CY-TAG if they

were currently enrolled in seventh through tenth grade and

received one of the following test scores as a seventh-grader;

> 20 on any ACT subtest, >430 on the SAT-Verbal, >500 on the

SAT-Math test, or > 930 for a combined SAT-Verbal and SAT-Math

score. Most of the students took the SAT or ACT as part of a

talent search. Students qualified for the talent search by

scoring in the top three percent in the nation on any

standardized achievement test.

The SAT includes a mathematical subtest (SAT-M) and a

verbal subtest (SAT-V), with both subtests having maximum

scores of 800. Minimum SAT scores earned by CY-TAG

participants, SAT-M = 500 or SAT-V » 430, are equal to the

average score earned by a college-bound high school senior

male, but five years later.

CY-TAG students attending during the summers of 1990,

1991, 1992, 1993, and 1994 and for whom SAT scores are

available were included in this study.

Governor's Institute. The students attending the

Governor's Institute were nominated by their junior high or

middle school as being their most talented student in one of

four areas: artistic design, archaeology, probability and

statistics, or environmental chemistry. At the Governor's

Institute they attended rigorous classes in one of those areas

Page 47: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

41

for six hours per day for three weeks. Over 700 nominations

were received each year. Out of those 700, 80 students were

chosen by selection committees that included educators,

professors, staff, students, and parents. The Governor's

Institute was held at Iowa State University during the summers

of 1990 and 1991. Although criteria for selection for the

Governor's Institute were different from selection criteria

for CY-TAG, their test scores proved to be equivalent to

scores received by CY-TAG participants. Only Governor's

Institute participants for whom SAT scores are available were

included in this study. This study used information collected

from Governor's Institute participants during 1990 and 1991.

Explorations 1 Students attending the one or two-week

Explorations 1 courses scored in the top three percent in the

nation or in the top seven percent of Iowa students on an

achievement test, such as the Iowa Test of Basic Skills.

Students can choose from a variety of courses designed to

provide intellectual stimulation and are provided with an

opportunity to explore a variety of social and recreational

activities on the Iowa State campus. Only Explorations I

students attending in 1992, 1993, or 1994 for which SAT scores

are available were included in this study. Although

Explorations I students did not have to meet the same stringent

admission criteria required of the CY-TAG students, most of

them had SAT scores equivalent to those received by CY-TAG

students.

Page 48: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

42

Students were classified as extremely mathematically

gifted if they scored in the top 20 percent of the subjects in

this study on the SAT-M. Students were classified as

extremely verbally gifted if they scored in the top 20 percent

of the subjects in this study on the SAT-V.

Classification of the primarily mathematically gifted or

primarily verbally gifted was done as follows: 1. 70 points

were added to the scores of all SAT-V scores so that the group

means of the SAT-V and SAT-M scores were close to being

equivalent. 2. Students having SAT-M scores 70 or more points

higher (about one standard deviation for their group) than

their SAT-V scores were classified as primarily-mathematically

gifted. 3. Students having SAT-V scores 70 or more points

higher (about one standard deviation for their group) than

their SAT-M scores were classified as primarily-verbally

gifted.

Instrumentation

The measures used in this study, in addition to their SAT

scores, included the Adjective Check List, the Study of

Values, the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire, and

the Strong Interest Inventory.

Ad-iactive Check List. The Adjective Check List (ACL;

Gough & Heilbrun, 1980) is a personality inventory consisting

of 300 adjectives or descriptive phrases. Students check

those words they believe to be self-descriptive. Based on

previous research, nine of the 37 ACL scales were selected to

Page 49: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

43

be used in this study. ACL scores available for this study

included those for CY-TAG students for all five years,

Governor's Institute participants for 1990 and 1991, and

Explorations 1 students for 1992, 1993, and 1994.

Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. The

Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire (MPQ) is a self-

administered personality inventory (Tellegen & Waller, 1982).

It consists of 300 binary items, most of which are answered

true or false. The measure yields 11 primary scales related to

content areas, three higher-order scales related to broader

traits, and six validity scales. The 11 primary scales were

used in this study. MPQ scores were available for 1992, 1993,

and 1994 subjects in this study.

Study of Values. The Study of Values (SOV; Allport,

Vernon, & Lindzey, 1970) is a self-administered inventory of

values based on the concept that individual personalities are

best understood by studying their values (Allport, Vernon, &

Lindzey, 1960). It consists of 120 controversial statements

or questions accompanied by alternative answers. Students

indicated their preference for the alternatives. The measure

yields a profile classifying the values into six categories;

Theoretical, Economic, Aesthetic, Social, Political, and

Religious. Scores were available for the CY-TAG participants

for all five years. Scores of Governor's Institute students

in 1990 and 1991 and Explorations 1 students in 1992, 1993, and

1994 also were included.

Page 50: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

44

Strong Interest Inventory. This research used the

current version of the Strong Interest Inventory (Harmon,

Hansen, Borgen, & Hcunmer, 1994) available from Consulting

Psychologists Press, Palo Alto, California. For the majority

of the items, test takers are asked to mark "like," "dislike,"

or "indifferent" to indicate their interest in a variety of

subjects, activities, and types of people. Other types of

items include asking the test takers to indicate preference

between two activities and indicating whether a statement is

descriptive of the person taking the test. The instrument

used includes some biographical items and some objectively-

scored questions about data, people, and things. The

instrument measures Holland's RIASEC themes (called General

Occupational Themes), 23 Basic Interest Scales reflecting

consistency of responses in specific areas, and 124

Occupational Scales based on similarity of responses to

responses of actual men and women in the various occupations.

This study used General Occupational Themes and six of the

Basic Interest Scales. Scores were available for participants

who attended one of the 1992, 1993, or 1994 prograons.

Procedure

Students attending the 1990 and 1991 summer programs

completed the measures during mass testing occurring during

the respective summer sessions. For the 1992, 1993 and 1994

programs, students completed the Adjective Check List,

Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire, Study of Values,

Page 51: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

45

and the Strong Interest Inventory and returned them by mail

before the respective sessions began.

Statistical Analysis

Data were analyzed using the SPSS computer program.

Statistical tests included two-tailed t-tests and discriminant

function analyses. For all analyses, an alpha level of .05

was used. For all analyses involving SAT-V and SAT-M, these

scores were age adjusted to be equivalent at the 7th grade

level.

For the t-tests, effect sizes (d « M1-M2/SD) were

computed to evaluate the magnitude of differences. According

to Cohen (1977), .20 is a small effect size, .50 is a medium

effect size, and .80 is a large effect size.

A final discriminant analysis of male subjects used the

variables that were significant at an alpha level of .05 on

the t-tests. When variables were correlated at .75 or higher,

only the variable with the highest alpha level was used in the

discriminant analysis. A similar discriminant analysis of

female subjects was carried out using variables that were

significant at an alpha level of .05 for the comparisons on

female subjects.

The power (probability of correctly rejecting a false

null hypothesis or the chance of finding an effect if it is

there) varies somewhat for the various comparisons because of

the variation in number of subjects for the various

instruments used. Power (using an alpha level of .05 and an

Page 52: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

46

effect size of .50) for the comparisons involving male

subjects is rather good/ ranging from .72 for comparisons

using the Strong to .87 for the comparisons using the Study of

Values. Power for the comparisons involving female subjects

is lower, ranging from .42 on the MPQ to .73 for the ACL.

RESULTS

This section will first describe, by gender, the

personality characteristics descriptive of gifted students in

general. In other words, the gifted boys will be compared to

male norm groups, and the gifted girls will be compared to

female norm groups. Next, significant gender differences will

be reported. Lastly, comparisons of the mathematically gifted

and the verbally gifted will be reported to see if and how

they might differ.

Personality Characteristics of Gifted Students

Males

Adjective Check List. The gifted adolescent boys were

compared to a norm group of 128 high school males on nine

selected scales of the Adjective Check List (ACL), but

differed substantially on only one scale. The gifted boys

scored higher on Self Confidence (M = 50.96, SD = 9.66, d =

.41) than did their high school peers (M = 46.70, SD = 10.28).

Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. Scores for

the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire (MPQ) were not

transformed into standard scores. Nonetheless, norms are

available for 500 college females and 300 college males.

Page 53: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

47

Comparing the gifted males to the older college males revealed

some noteworthy differences between means. The most startling

was on Alienation. The gifted boys were much higher on

Alienation (M = 8.05, SD = 5.13, d = 1.39) than the college

norm group (M = 2.9, SD = 3.7). They were also higher on

Aggression (M = 10.98, SD •* 5.13, d => .87) compared to the

college sample (M » 7.0, SD = 4.6), as well as lower on Social

Closeness (M= 12.43, SD= 4.71, d= .59) than the normative

group (M = 15.2, SD =• 4.7). As a group, the gifted adolescent

boys appear to experience more problems with human

relationships than college males.

General Occupational Themes. The six General

Occupational Themes (GOT) on the Strong are the same as

Holland's RIASEC themes. When compared to a group of 77

entering college males (median age of 18.6 years; Lapan,

Boggs, & Morrill, 1989), the gifted adolescent boys in this

study scored lower on all of the GOT themes except

Investigative, on which the gifted males (M = 53.55, SD =

8.11) scored somewhat higher than the college males (M = 50.8,

SD 10.4, d = .26). The biggest difference was on the

Enterprising scale: The gifted adolescent boys scored much

lower (M = 43.29, SD = 9.40, d = .86) than the college

freshmen (M = 51.4, SD = 9.4). Additionally, the gifted boys

scored much lower on Social, (M = 39.41, SD = 10.11, d = .62)

than the college seimple (M = 46.0, SD = 10.6), lower on

iVrtistic, (M = 43.21, SD = 10.58, d = .48) than the college

Page 54: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

48

men (M = 48.3, SD = 10.7), and showed less interest in

Conventional occupations (M • 46.67, SD = 9.81, d = .60) than

did the college males (M » 52.8, SD » 10.3). As a group, the

gifted adolescent boys strongly preferred occupations that

offered the challenge of using their minds to solve problems.

Basic Interest Scales. On the six selected Basic

Interest Scales (BIS) of the Strong, the norms available are

for adult males (average age of 38; Campbell, 1977).

Nevertheless, there are noteworthy differences on three of

those scales: Adventure, Nature, and Religion. Gifted males

showed higher interest in Adventure (M = 57.09, SD = 9.24, d =

.45) than the older males (M = 52.7, SD = 9.7), but lower

interest in Nature (M = 41.26, SD = 9.15, d = .81) than the

adult men (M = 49.3, SD = 9.9), and less interest in Religious

Activities (M = 43.09, SD = 9.94, d = .66) than the older norm

group (M = 49.6, SD = 9.9). Overall, the gifted boys appear

to be more sensation-seeking and less interested in spiritual

matters than the adult norm group. Whether these interests

will change with age cannot be determined from the data.

Study of Values. When the gifted adolescent males in

this study were compared on the Study of Values (SOV) using

norms derived from 10th grade boys, the biggest difference was

that the gifted boys were much lower on the value of Religion

(M = 32.45, SD = 10.88, d = .62) than their peers (M = 37.09

SD, = 7.55). The gifted males also scored higher on Aesthetic

values (M = 37.61, SD = 8.11, d= .36) than the lOth-graders

Page 55: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

49

(M - 35.00, SD » 7.44) as well as higher on Theoretical values

(M - 47.59, SD « 7.06, d " .46) than the normative 10th grade

boys (M = 44.59, SD = 6.48).

Females

Adjective Check List. Using nine selected scales of the

ACL, the gifted girls in this study were compared to a norm

group of 126 high school females. Like the gifted boys, the

girls scored substantially higher on Self Confidence (M =

51.14, SD = 10.46, d = .62) than their high school peers (M =

45.87, SD = 8.56). The gifted girls also were substantially

higher on Endurance (M = 47.67, SD = 8.54, d «= .57), than the

high school sample (M » 42.40, SD >• 8.83). Differences

between the groups on Deference and Order were less

substantial. The gifted group was higher on Order (M = 46.26,

SD = 9.84, d = .34) than the normative group (M « 43.17, SD =

9.09) and lower on Deference (M = 44.37, SD = 10.59, d = .35)

than the high school females (M = 47.75, SD = 9.70). Thus,

the gifted adolescent females in this study can be described

as having high self-confidence, being persistent in pursuing

their goals, placing importance on planning and organization,

and unwilling to play subordinate roles in their

relationships.

Multidimensional Personalitv Questionnaire. The gifted

adolescent girls were compared to a norm group of 500 college

females on the MPQ. Like the gifted boys, gifted girls scored

much higher on Alienation (M = 5.47, SD = 4.25, d = 1.11) than

Page 56: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

50

the college norm group (M = 2.0, SD 2.7). They also were

lower on Harm Avoidance (M = 13.54, SD = 6.52) than the

college females (17.0, SD = 5.7, d = .61). The gifted girls

appear more willing to to take risks, but tend to be more

cynical and distrustful regarding personal relationships.

General Occupation Themes. When the gifted adolescent

girls were compared to a group of 71 incoming college women

(median age of 18.6; Lapan, Boggs, & Morrill, 1989) on the six

scales of the GOT, they differed the most from the college

group in their interest in Investigative occupations. The

gifted girls scored much higher on Investigative (M = 52.79,

SD = 8.92, d = .74) than the college women (M = 45.2, SD =

10.2). They also differed substantially on the Enterprising

and Conventional themes. The gifted adolescent girls scored

lower on the Enterprising theme (M = 44.76, SD = 9.85, d =

.78) than the normative group (M « 51.8, SD 9.0) and were also

less interested in Conventional occupations (M = 47.13, SD =

10.25, d = .59) than the college freshmen (M = 53.4, SD =

10.6). As a group, the gifted girls demonstrated greatest

interest in Artistic (M = 54.03, SD = 9.25) and Investigative

occupations. They differed most from the norms in their high

interest in Investigative careers and in their low interest in

occupations involving Enterprising or Conventional tasks.

Basic Interest Scales. The available female norms for

BIS scales were derived from 300 adult females (average age of

38; Ceunpbell, 1977). The gifted adolescent girls differed

Page 57: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

51

substantially from the older women on only one scale: They

were higher on Adventure (M « 51.28, SD •« 10.01, d - .42) than

the adult females (M = 47.3, SD » 9.5).

Study of Values. On the SOV the gifted adolescent girls

were compared to norms for 10th grade females. Like the boys,

the gifted girls differed most from their peers in their low

interest in Religious values, (M = 36.10, SD 9.93, d = .85),

scoring much lower than the normative 10th grade girls (M =•

43.11, SD «= 8.29). They also were much higher on Aesthetic (M

= 43.72, SD = 7.73, d = .80) compared to their peers (M =

38.02, SD « 7.13) and somewhat higher on Theoretical (M =

41.06, SD = 7.19, d = .48) than the normative 10th graders (M

= 37.80, SD = 6.80). In comparison to peers, the gifted girls

like to use their minds to solve problems and increase their

understanding of what is going on, but they also appreciate

aesthetics. They are not much interested in Religious issues.

Overall, the gifted boys and girls in this study showed

relatively high self-confidence when compared to normative

groups, a finding that is especially interesting considering

that they also reported substantial feelings of alienation.

The gifted males and females also were much more interested

than comparison groups in using their minds to understand the

world and solve problems. The gifted groups demonstrated a

higher need for excitement and adventure, but lower interest

in religious matters than older comparison groups. The gifted

boys showed less interest in personal relationships than the

Page 58: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

52

comparison groups, but that finding did not hold for the

gifted girls.

Gender Differences

Adjective Check List. Of the nine selected scales of the

ACL used in this study, only one showed a statistically

significant gender difference. The gifted girls were

negligibly higher on Endurance, t(542) = 2.20, £< .05, d=

.19.

Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. Seven of the

11 scales of the MPQ showed statistically significant

differences between gifted males and females. The gifted

girls were higher on Weil-Being, t(331) = 2.05, e < .05, d =

.23, Social Closeness, t{331) = 2.52, p < .05, d = .28, Stress

Reaction, t(331) = 2.22, p < .001, d = .25, Harm Avoidance,

t(331) = 4.12, E < .001, d = .45, and Absorption, t(331)

4.80, p < .001, d = .53. The gifted boys were higher on

Alienation, t(331) = 4.76, p < .001, d = .52, and Aggression,

t{331) = 9.23, p < .001, d = .93. Although they did report

more anxiety than their male counterparts, the gifted girls,

as a whole, appeared to feel more optimistic and personally

connected to other people, and less prone to antisocial or

dangerous activity.

General Occupational Themes. Three of the six GOT scales

showed sizable gender differences, with the gifted males

higher on Realistic, t(329) «= 4.43, p < .001, d = .48, and the

Page 59: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

53

gifted females higher on Artistic, t(329) = 9.49, £ < .001, d

= .95, and Social, t(329) ** 8.45, £ < .001, d » .86.

Basic Interest Scales. All of the six BIS scales

examined showed sizable gender differences. Gifted females

demonstrated much higher interest in Music, t(329), £ < 11.24,

d = 1.08, Art, t{329) = 9.69, e < .001, d = .96, Nature,

t{329) = 8.63, E < .001, d = .88, and Religion, t(329) = 4.33,

E < .001, d = .48, but scored significantly lower on

Adventure, t(329) = 5.38, e < .001., d = .58, and Athletics,

t(329) =2.88, '01/ .32. Interests of this gifted

population varied sizably by gender. The girls showed more

appreciation of aesthetics, while the boys showed considerably

more interest in sensation-seeking activities.

Study of Values. The gifted adolescent girls were quite

different from the boys in their values. There were

significant gender differences on all six of the SOV scales.

The boys were much higher on Theoretical. t(528) = 10.40, e <

.001, d = .84, Economic, t(528) = 9.76, e .001, d = .79, and

Political values, t(528) = 7.33, e < .001, d = .62. The

girls, on the other hand, were much higher on Aesthetic,

t(528) = 8.70, E < -OOlf d = .65, Social, t(528) = 11.15, e <

.001, d = .89, and Religious values, t(528) = 3.95, e < .001,

d = .34.

The gifted adolescents in this study demonstrated some

consistent gender differences. The girls showed more

enjoyment of people and of the aesthetic experiences of art.

Page 60: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

54

music, and nature. They were also more interested in

religious and spiritual issues and experiences (but less so

than girls in general). The boys, on the other hand, showed

more interest in the physical world. They were more

practical, desire physical sensation and adventure, and were

more instrumental in their desire to make things happen and

understand the physical world.

Comparisons of Mathematically and Verbally-Gifted Students

Males

Comparisons of the characteristics of mathematically and

verbally gifted boys were done in two ways. In one set of

comparisons, boys scoring in the top 20 percent on the SAT-

Math were compared with those scoring in the top 20 percent of

the SAT-Verbal. In another set of comparisons, the groups

were made up of boys who had significant discrepancies (of

approximately one standard deviation) in their talents. The

primarily-mathematically gifted group had much higher SAT-Math

scores; the primarily-verbally gifted group had much higher

SAT-Verbal scores. The results of these two types of

comparisons were quite similar, suggesting that it may not be

the level of precocity but rather the effect of discrepancy of

abilities within the individual that is related to personality

traits among the highly gifted. The vast majority of the top

20 percent math group (96%) was also included in the

primarily-mathematically gifted group, making up about one

half of the primarily-mathematical group. Similarly, most of

Page 61: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

55

the top 20 percent verbal students (73%) were also in the

primarily-verbally gifted group and made up about one half of

the primarily-verbal group. Because the results of the

comparisons were so similar/ only the results of the

comparisons between the primarily-mathematically gifted and

the primarily-verbally gifted are presented. It must be

emphasized that although these students have significant

discrepancies in their abilities, all were highly

intellectually gifted.

Ad-iective Check List. Using nine selected scales of the

Adjective Check List (ACL), personality characteristics of the

primarily-mathematically gifted were compared with those of

the primarily-verbally gifted to determine whether there were

differences in how the two groups describe themselves. Three

of the scales showed significant differences between the

groups. The primarily-verbally gifted boys were higher on

Autonomy, t(169) « 2.19, e < *05, d = .32, but lower on

Deference, t(169) » 2.24, e < '05, d = .34, and Self-Control,

t(169) = 2.05, E 'OSf d = .33 (see Figure 1).

Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. Of the 11

MPQ scales, four significantly discriminated between the two

groups: The primarily-verbally gifted boys were higher on

Aggression, t(109) = 2.08, n < '05, d = .41, but lower on

Control, t(109) * 1.99, e .05, d = .41, Harm Avoidance,

t(109) =2.41, E< .05, d= .45, and Traditionalism, t(109) =

3.14, E < .05 d = .57 (see Figure 2).

Page 62: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

56

General Occupational Themes. Of the six GOT scales, only-

Conventional, t(106) » 2.40, E < .05, d « .49, significantly

discriminated between the two groups; the primarily-

mathematically gifted boys scored higher (see Figure 3).

Basic Interest Scales. Of the six selected BIS scales,

only Adventure, t(106) =2.15, £< .05, d= .43, significantly

discriminated between the two groups. The primarily-verbally

gifted boys were higher on their desire for adventure (see

Figure 4).

Study of Values. Using the SOV, the values of boys whose

talents are primarily mathematical were compared with those

whose talents are primarily verbal to see how values may

differentiate the two groups at this age. Of the six SOV

scales, only the Aesthetic scale was significant, t(165) =

5.01, p < .001, d = .77, with primarily-verbal boys

demonstrating much more interest in aesthetic values (see

Figure 5).

Overall, some interesting differences emerged. As

compared to the primarily-mathematically gifted boys, the

primarily-verbally gifted boys were more adventurous and risk-

taking, constantly pushing limits of any kind. They are more

willing to aggressively insist on doing things "their way."

Other than the differences just discussed, it is in the area

of vocational interests and values that differences by talent

domain are most obvious.

Page 63: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

57

Final Discriminant Analysis. A final discriminant

analysis was done, using variables that had been significant

at the .05 level or lower. Variables used for males in the

final discriminant analysis were Deference (ACL), Control

(MPQ), Aggression (MPQ), Harm Avoidance (MPQ), Traditionalism

(MPQ), Conventional (GOT), Adventure (BIS), and Aesthetic

(SOV). Two variables were discarded. Autonomy (ACL) and Self-

Control (ACL), because of high correlations (r = .75 or above)

with other variables used in the final discriminant analysis.

The resulting discriminant functions were significant (£

< .05) with a canonical r of .41. This function correctly

classified 67 percent of the gifted males as primarily

mathematical or primarily verbal compared to 51 percent that

would be classified correctly by chance. The most powerful

variables in the discriminant function were Aesthetic,

Conventional, and Adventure (see Table 1).

Females

The number of female subjects for some of the groups is

small, especially for comparisons using the MPQ and the

Strong. They were, therefore, examined only for trends and to

see whether the results were similar to those for males.

Although there are important average differences between

profiles of interests and values between the males and the

females, the pattern of personality differences between the

primarily-mathematically gifted and the primarily verbally

gifted adolescents was similar in most cases across gender.

Page 64: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

58

In other words, the means on the personality instruments often

differed by gender^ but the relationships between the primary

talent area and personality were quite constant across males

and females (see Figures 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5).

Table 1

Results from Final Discriminant Analysis for Males Using Variables Significant at a .05 Level on T-Tests

Standardized Variables Weights

Deference .17 Control .15 Aggression .11 Harm Avoidance -.15 Traditionalism -.15 Conventional -.43 Adventure .42 Aesthetic .72

Note. Canonical r= .41; e< .05.

General Occupational Themes. Of the six GOT scales, the

primarily-verbally gifted girls scored significantly higher

compared to the primarily-mathematically gifted girls on

Artistic, t(70) = 3.12, e < 'Ol/ d = .73, but lower on the

Conventional, t(70) =2.29, E < .05, d = .61. These results

are consistent with the comparisons done on the males.

Primarily-verbally gifted adolescents of both genders scored

lower on the Conventional scale than the primarily-

mathematically gifted. On Artistic, only the females

demonstrated a statistically significant difference; verbal

Page 65: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

59

males, nevertheless, were also higher on Artistic (see Figure

3).

Basic Interest Scales. On the six selected BIS scales,

primarily-verbally gifted girls scored significantly higher on

Art, t(70) = 2.46, e < .05, d = .62, a finding similar to that

for the boys. On the remaining scales, when there were

significant differences, they were in the seone direction for

both males and females (see Figure 4).

Study of Values. Except for the Religious scale on the

SOV, the scales showing statistically significant differences

between the math and verbal groups were again in the same

direction for both genders (see Figure 5). These differences

are as follows. The math group was significantly higher on

Theoretical, t(122) =2.19, e .05, d = .44, and Economic

t{122) = 3.21, .01, d= .63, but lower on Aesthetic,

t(122) = 2.28, E < .05, d = .43, and Religious, t(122) = 2.16,

E < .05, d = .41.

The Religious scale on the SOV is the only scale in this

study where there was a significant difference between the

verbal group and the math group, but direction of the

difference varied by gender. The verbal girls were

significantly higher on Religious values than math girls,

t(122) = 2.16, E < .05, d = .41. Although the difference for

the male comparison is not statistically significant (3.27

points), it is the math boys who scored higher than the verbal

boys on Religious values. It must be noted that neither

Page 66: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

60

gender had high absolute scores on the Religious values.

Additionally/ the standard deviation (10.64) for the Religious

scale was higher for the gifted group as a whole than it was

for other scales on the SOV, suggesting that this gifted

population shows the most variability on the Religious scale.

Findings are in need of replication, especially here.

Final Discriminant Analvses. Variables used for females

in the final discriminant analysis were the ones that had been

significant at a .05 level or lower for the female comparisons

and were somewhat different from the ones used in the final

discriminant analysis for males. Variables used for females

were Artistic (GOT), Conventional (GOT), Theoretical (SOV),

Aesthetic (SOV), Religious (SOV), and Economic (SOV). Art

(BIS) was discarded because of its high correlation (r = .92)

with Artistic.

The resulting discriminant function was significant (e <

.001) with a canonical r of .54. This function correctly

classified 78 percent of the females as primarily mathematical

or primarily verbal compared to 63 percent that would be

correctly classified by chance. The most powerful variables

were Artistic, Conventional, and Theoretical (see Table 2).

The Conventional theme on the GOT significantly

discriminates between the primarily mathematical and primarily

verbal for both genders. Although the direction of

differences between primarily-mathematically and primarily

Page 67: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

61

Table 2

Results from Final Discriminant Analysis for Females using Variables Significant at a .05 Level

Standardized Variables Weights

Theoretical .46 Artistic -.63 Conventional .60 Economic -.18 Aesthetic .01 Religious -.24

Note. E < .001; canonical r = .54.

verbally-gifted students students are sdLmilar for both males

and females, the amount of difference does vary by gender.

DISCUSSION

Intellectually gifted people have a highly-developed need

to "know." Although this compulsion to make sense out of the

world was even more remarkable in the boys, both the

adolescent boys and the adolescent girls in this study

demonstrated, by their high scores on the Theoretical scale of

the SOV and the Investigative scale on the Holland's RIASEC

themes (as measured by GOT on the Strong), that this is a

greater need for them than it is for their peers. This

finding is not at all new. It is akin to the curiosity named

by Cox (1981) as "one of most pervasive characteristics" (p.

113) of intellectually-gifted children and is related to their

need for mental stimulation described by Lovecky (1992). It

can be understood as being connected to their high ability to

Page 68: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

62

think abstractly and to see relationships quickly. These

children tend to be analytical and critical thinkers who need

to think through problems until they understand how all the

pieces fit together. Both the gifted males and gifted females

in this study showed less interest in the Enterprising and

Conventional themes of the GOT than the normative groups, a

finding that is also related to the "need to know" so

characteristic of this population. Many of the Enterprising

and Conventional occupations do not offer the opportunity for

sustained intellectual activity that gifted individuals want

in their careers.

Another characteristic descriptive of the adolescents in

this study, but one less intuitively related to their

intellectual ability, is a sense of alienation. Although both

the boys and girls scored high on the Alienation scale on the

MPQ in relation to the normative groups, the boys' sense of

alienation appeared greater than that of the girls. Again,

this finding is not new. Janos, Fung, and Robinson (1985)

found that many gifted children perceive themselves to be

"different" from their peers and that such children have lower

self-esteem. Such perceptions by these children are not

imaginary. Their differences in both interests and vocabulary

(Hollingworth, 1942) set them apart and contribute to

difficulty in finding compatible friends (Greenlaw & Mcintosh,

1988). In return, their peers also perceive them as different

and, at times, tend to exclude them (Janos, Fung, & Robinson,

Page 69: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

63

1985). This alienation also can be fostered by an insistence

on making their own judgments (Erode, 1980) and astonishment

that everybody does not agree with their conclusions.

The relatively high self-confidence demonstrated by both

the girls and boys in this study is somewhat perplexing,

considering their high scores on alienation. The self-

confidence of intellectually-gifted students is given mixed

reviews in the literature. Purkey (1966) did not find gifted

adolescents to have more self-confidence than average-ability

peers, and Sheldon (1959) found highly-gifted children to lack

confidence. Werner and Bachtold (1969) found adolescent boys

(but not adolescent girls) to be high in self-assurance.

Others (Gottsdanker, 1968; Warren & Heist, 1960) have

concluded that highly-gifted individuals are confident.

To put the confidence of these adolescents in

perspective, one should consider the context. Bollinger

(1983) argued that self-concept is not unitary, but rather a

composite of self-impressions. The same could be argued for

self-confidence. Intellectually-gifted students have been

found to have positive academic self-concepts (Kelly &

Colangelo, 1984; Ross & Parker, 1980; Swiatek, 1993). It is

likely that confidence in academic pursuits at least partially

accounts for the high self-confidence scores for the subjects

in this study.

It is informative that both adolescent girls and

adolescent boys rated lowest on Religious values on the SOV.

Page 70: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

64

To fully understand this finding it must be considered that

the SOV scores are ipsative—that is, a high score on one or

more scales requires that scores on other scales must be low,

and, according to Warren and Heist (1960), high Theoretical or

Aesthetic scores often depress the Religious scale. The

research literature is not entirely consistent on the

importance of Religious values, as measured by the SOV, for

people who are highly intelligent. Southern and Plant (1968)

found Mensa members to score extremely low on Religious

values, and other researchers (Fox & Denham, 1974; McGinn,

1976) have found gifted adolescents to score very low on the

Religious scale. On the other hand. Warren and Heist (1960)

found that National Merit Scholars scored higher than their

peers on Religious values. For the students in this study,

however, the very low scores on Religious values are

substantiated by similar low scores on Religious interests on

the BIS scales of the Strong.

Gifted boys in this study scored low on the Social

Closeness scale on the MPQ, suggesting a preference for being

alone and working out problems without help from others. This

is very similar to the introversion that many researchers have

found to be characteristic of highly-intelligent people

(Berndt, Kaiser, & van Aalst, 1982; Haier & Denhemi, 1976;

Silverman, 1993) and is also related to the isolation and

alienation discussed earlier. This finding also supports

their very low score on the Social theme of the GOT scales of

Page 71: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

65

the Strong. Low scores by gifted males on the Social theme

have been consistently found in research literature (Lubinski,

Benbov, & Sanders, 1993; Fox & Denham, 1974; Haier & Denham,

1974). This does not mean that these males are socially

inept. In fact, the research by Haier and Denheun (1974)

suggests that they have better than average social skills. It

does suggest, however, that they have a preference for

spending their time in activities requiring mental challenge

to those involving social relationships with people. This

lack of interest in people and personal relationships is not

at all true of the intellectually-gifted girls in this study.

Both gifted boys and gifted girls scored higher than

peers on the Aesthetic scale of the SOV, supporting earlier

findings that gifted people have "high levels of aesthetic

awareness and appreciation" (p. 335, Warren & Heist, 1960).

To put this in perspective, however, it must be pointed out

that the gifted boys (in contrast to the girls) did not value

Aesthetics highly—just more highly than the lOth-grade males

in the normative group. Additionally, the boys scored lower

than the comparison group on Artistic (GOT) and Nature (BIS).

In both cases the comparison groups were somewhat older. It

is possible that the sensory experiences offered by the arts

and by nature do not provide sufficient mental stimulation to

capture the interest of gifted males of this age.

The high score by the gifted adolescent boys on the

Aggression scale of the MPQ was rather unexpected. Aggression

Page 72: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

66

is not frequently associated with the gifted population. In

fact, research examining the relationship has found gifted

children not to be more aggressive than their peers (e.g.

Lehman & Erdwins, 1981; Liddle, 1958; Loeb & Jay, 1987; Ludwig

& Cullinan, 1984). The subjects in the four studies just

cited were elementary age, and it is possible that the gifted

males become more aggressive relative to their peers after

puberty. The Aggression scale of the MPQ does appear to

measure a type of aggression associated with hostility, and

this unexpected finding may be related to the high level of

alienation reported by the same subjects.

Gifted girls in this study scored lower than normative

groups on Harm Avoidance (MPQ) and higher on Adventure (BIS),

suggesting a risk-taking attitude described by some

researchers (Davids, 1966; Payne, Halpin, Ellett, & Dale,

1975). They also scored higher than the normative group on

Endurance (ACL), supporting the notion that highly-intelligent

students persevere, even when the going gets rough (Hildreth,

1938; Wall, 1960).

That gifted adolescent girls demonstrated less Deference

(ACL) than the normative group is certainly no surprise. The

unwillingness of gifted individuals of either gender to submit

to the dictates of those in authority is well documented

(Badgett, Fair, & Hunkler, 1974, Mason, Adams, & Blood, 1968).

Their higher score on Order, suggesting organization and

planning, is less easily explained. It does fit in with the

Page 73: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

67

perseverance described earlier and is consistent with the

thorough planning described by Brandwein (1955).

Overall, the interests of both gifted males and gifted

females in this study appear dominated by their need for

mental challenge. This thirst for knowledge, which is a

dominant force in their values and their choice of life work,

is more intense for the males. For the females, it is

tempered by a greater enjoyment of social activities and

appreciation of aesthetic pursuits. The self-confidence of

this population in spite of the alienation they feel is an

interesting finding. Additionally, the males scored somewhat

higher than the norm group on aggression, and the females

appear to be more risk-taking, perseverant, organized, but

less submissive to authority than the normative groups.

Gender Differences

Males and females have different interests and values.

Thorndike (1911) summed up an important distinction when he

pointed out that men are highly interested in things, but

women are most interested in people. For the highly-gifted

population, the difference might better be conceptualized as

people versus ideas. The gifted adolescent males in this

study love to play with ideas, as evidenced by their high

Theoretical (SOV) and Investigative (GOT) scores. It is not

that the gifted girls are not interested in ideas, but that

they have competing interests; They enjoy and value people,

and they like pursuing aesthetic experiences. The adolescent

Page 74: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

68

girls in this study consistently scored higher on all scales

related to social activities^ with higher scores on the Social

scales of both the SOV and the GOT and with a higher score on

Social Closeness of the MPQ. Additionally, the gifted boys

demonstrated higher Alienation (MFQ), another indicator that

they have more trouble getting close to people than the girls

do.

The greater appreciation of beauty in all forms by gifted

females was demonstrated in higher scores on Aesthetic (SOV),

Artistic (GOT), Art (BIS), and Nature (BIS). Their

significantly higher score on Absorption (MPQ) can be viewed

as additional evidence of their love of beauty in that it

describes their ability to be caught up or enthralled by

sensory experiences. None of these findings are new. High

interest by gifted males on theoretical-investigative

interests and of the females on social and aeshetics pursuits

are well documented (Astin, 1974; Lubinski, Benbow, & Sanders,

1993; Southern & Plant, 1968; Warren & Heist, 1960).

The girls also were consistent in their higher value of

religion, scoring higher on the Religious scales on both the

SOV and the BIS. This finding supports the results of earlier

studies using the SOV in which gifted females repeatedly

scored higher than males in religious interests (Lubinski,

Benbow, & Sanders, 1993; Southern & Plant, 1968; Warren &

Heist, 1960). The gifted boys also scored higher than the

girls on the Economic scale of the SOV and on the Realistic

Page 75: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

69

theme of the GOT, supporting Thorndike's assertion that males

show more interest in things and consistent with previous

studies using the SOV and GOT (Lubinski, Benbow, & Sanders^

1993; Southern & Plant, 1968; Warren & Heist, 1960).

The adolescent boys also scored significantly higher on

the Political scale of the SOV. The Political scale deals

with interest in power and is akin to Aggression (MFQ), which

relates to the willingness to use power or force and may be

connected to the higher activity level typically found in

males (Erne, 1979). Gender differences relating to power and

aggression are among the most highly researched differences

between males and females in the general population and

certainly are not limited to the gifted population (Hyde,

1990). The higher interest in Athletics (BIS) demonstrated by

the males can also be tied to a greater willingness by males

to use power for advantage (Jacklin, 1992), to a more

competitive spirit (Arch, 1993), and to the higher activity

level mentioned earlier (Eme, 1979).

Archer and Lloyd (1985) pointed out a salient

relationship between aggression and fear when they stated,

"Fear inhibits aggression, and aggression inhibits fear" (p.

160). Consistent with that relationship between fear and

aggression, the gifted boys scored significantly higher on

aggression but lower on Stress Reaction (MFQ), a scale that

relates to worry and negative mood states. In this respect

the gifted adolescents appear to mirror the general

Page 76: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

70

population, in which adolescent females reportedly experience

more anxiety (Bernstein, Garfinkel, & Hoberman, 1989) and

considerably more depression than males (Cohen, et al., 1993).

Burke and Weir (1978) concluded that adolescent girls

experience more stress than boys and, therefore, are under

more psychological strain, while Archer and Lloyd (1985)

offered the possible explanation that they may be more willing

to admit their distress. It is noteworthy that regardless of

their distress, the gifted females still scored higher on

Weil-Being (MPQ), a scale that suggests optimism and

happiness. It can be posited that the support that the

females experience from close relationships neutralizes the

stress they experience and allows them to enjoy their lives.

The greater risk-taking willingness demonstrated by the

gifted boys in this study by their lower score on Harm

Avoidance (MPQ) is also characteristic of males and females in

general (Block, 1983). Arch (1993) posited that females are

also less willing to take risks in the social arena, another

indication of the importance of social relationships to

females. One is less likely to risk something that is very

precious. The gifted adolescent girls also were higher on

Endurance (ACL), suggesting a greater willingness to

persevere. This finding, when coupled with the higher scores

by females on Harm Avoidance (MPQ), suggests that the gifted

girls are more inclined to persist on tedious tasks, but not

when risk is involved.

Page 77: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

71

Overall, the main distinctions between the gifted males

and females in this study are the males' compelling need to

play with ideas and their greater preoccupation with things.

The females, while they enjoy mental challenge, also value and

enjoy people more than the males and are more anxious to

pursue aesthetic interests. They also are more interested in

religious and spiritual activities. The adolescent girls

reported more psychological distress, but also appeared more

successful in building close relationships, with the overall

result of a sense of well-being that is somewhat higher than

that of the males.

Differences according to Talent Domain

The gifted adolescents in this study clearly have

personality patterns related to their talents or areas of

intellectual strength. The overall distinction between the

mathematical group and verbal group of gifted adolescents

appears to be an aversion, by the verbal group, to any type of

boundaries or limitations, as will become evident as each area

of distinction is examined more closely.

Autonomy and Deference

The primarily-verbally gifted boys scored higher on

Autonomy (ACL), but lower on Deference (ACL). People high on

autonomy act independently of the actions or wishes of others

and certainly are less likely to demonstrate respect for

authority. This finding supports earlier research that

suggests verbally-gifted individuals tend to be more

Page 78: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

72

individualistic (Werner & Bachtold, 1969), less conforming,

and "aloof from higher authority" (Sanders, Mefferd, & Bown,

1960, p. 501). They also have been described as relying on

their own perceptions and feelings to make their decisions

(Sanders, Mefferd, & Bown, 1960). D'Heurle, Mellinger, and

Haggard (1959) found students who were high achievers in

reading but not math to be less responsive to parental

pressure and control.

The Traditionalism scale of the MPQ, which measures how

much a person subscribes to conventional values, is closely

related to this wish to be free of constraints and authority.

Consistent with results on Autonomy and Deference, the

primarily-verbal boys also scored lower than the primarily-

mathematical boys on the Traditionalism scale.

Aesthetics

The primarily-verbally gifted boys scored higher than the

primarily-mathematically gifted boys in their Aesthetic

values (SOV). This can be tied to the observations of other

researchers that verbally-talented individuals have a more

subjective world-view, while the mathematically-talented tend

toward a more objective view of reality (D'Heurle, Mellinger,

& Haggard, 1959; Sanders, Mefferd, & Bown, 1960). Viernstein,

McGinn, and Hogan (1977) found verbal individuals to be more

responsive to sensory experience. Additionally, the verbal

males preferred graphic designs that were asymmetrical

complex, and unfinished, in contrast to the mathematical boys

Page 79: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

73

who preferred organized, syinmetrical, and simple designs. The

verbally gifted are also more prone to fantasy (D'Heurle,

Mellinger, & Haggard, 1959). Thus, it can be argued that the

greater interest demonstrated by the verbally-gifted boys for

aesthetic values is additional evidence of their disdain for

limits and of their need to express themselves with the

symbols of a subjective reality.

Control

In keeping with their dislike of limits, the verbal group

scored lower on Self-Control (ACL) and Control (MPQ), scales

that measure such attributes as impulsivity and dependability.

This finding also supports earlier research, which found

mathematically-gifted individuals to be more conscientious and

rule-oriented (Werner & Bachtold, 1969), less distractible

(Viernstein, McGinn, & Hogan, 1977; Maccoby & Rau, 1962), and

to demonstrate more endurance and perseverance (McCarthy,

1975; Sanders, Mefferd, & Bown, 1960). People who score high

on the Conventional scale (GOT) of the Strong are described as

highly ordered, stable, dependable, and well-controlled. This

is similar to the Self-Control (ACL) and Control (MPQ) scales.

The lower scores by the verbal group on Conventional (GOT)

support the impression that verbally-talented individuals are

more impulsive, careless, and less dependable than the

mathematical group. It seems that the verbally-gifted boys do

not like limits, even when self-imposed.

Page 80: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

74

Aggression

The primarily-verbally gifted boys scored statistically-

significantly higher than the primarily-mathematically gifted

on the Aggression scale of the MPQ. On the Aggression scale

of ACL, the difference approached significance (g = .068) in

the scune direction. These findings suggest more willingness

by the high verbal boys to use force to get what they want and

support the impression that the high verbal adolescent males

are more willing to push the limits in general.

Information in the literature about aggression as

relating to intellectual giftedness and talent domains is

meager as well as complex. Some studies have found math-

talented boys to be more aggressive (Ferguson & Maccoby,

1966). Others have found the verbally-talented boys to be

more aggressive (McCarthy, 1975). The stickler seems to be

that aggression can be manifested in several ways. Ferguson

and Maccoby (1966) posited that mathematical ability is

related to a more appropriate or productive type of

aggression, while the high verbal boys display a more

antisocial aggression. This conclusion is supported by

findings that high-verbal children are more hostile (D'Heurle,

Mellinger, & Haggard, 1959; Sanford, 1943) as well as the

results of Ferguson and Maccoby's (1966) study indicating that

high verbal boys reported more antisocial and hostile feelings

but that peers rated high math boys higher on "the kind of

aggression that seems appropriate for boys of this age in

Page 81: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

75

their interaction with peers" (p. 568). The type of

aggression measured by the MFQ in this study appears to be

more of the hostile type, and, in fact, correlates (r = .41)

with the Alienation (MPQ) scale.

The above discussion about personality differences

related to talent domain pertains to the gifted adolescent

boys in this study. As related earlier, the Religious value

of the SOV is the only scale in the study in which, when a

statistically-significant difference emerged, the differences

were not in the same direction for each gender. In this case,

only the difference for the females was statistically

significant. (Verbally-gifted females were higher than

mathematically-gifted females on Religious values.) An

examination of correlations, by gender, between mathematical

talent and verbal talent with the Religious scale (SOV)

reveals very small correlations, suggesting that this finding

might not be replicated in further research.

In considering all the relationships examined in this

study, it does appear that the inclination of the verbally-

gifted adolescent males to push the limits and insist on

"doing it my way" puts them at more social and psychological

risk than the other gifted adolescents. Indeed, the

literature supports some negative social and psychological

traits associated with high verbal ability. Individuals with

high verbal ability tend to be less sociable and outgoing,

less confident, and more withdrawn, tense, and anxious

Page 82: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

76

(D'Heurle, Mellinger, & Haggard, 1959). It is quite likely

that their more extreme individualism, plus a vocabulary that

sets them apart from their peers, hinders social relationships

and encourages a sense of alienation that can lead to

aggressive and antisocial behavior. Being female appears to

offer some protection in that gifted boys have more antisocial

and aggressive behavior than gifted girls (Ludwig & Cullinan,

1984). Protective mechanisms may include a better ability to

develop close relationships demonstrated by the gifted

adolescent females in this study. The gifted verbal girls do

have special risks of their own, however. A study by Jensen

(1994) indicated that verbally-gifted girls had high levels of

depression and paranoia.

The results of this study highlight the alienation and

loneliness experienced by many intellectually-gifted

adolescents and draw attention to intensified risk of

verbally-gifted males for social problems that can lead to

risky or even antisocial behavior. Parents and educators need

to be informed of these risks so that they can create

opportunities for these students to express themselves

individualistically within a framework that is accepting and

also offer psychological support when needed.

There are several major limitations to this study. One

is the lack of good normative groups for the comparisons of

the gifted adolescents with non-gifted peers. This is

especially salient for the comparisons using BIS scales, where

Page 83: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

77

the only available comparison groups were adults with an

average age of 38. Such comparisons obviously do not control

for changes that occur as people get older, and, thus, are

quite limited in their usefulness.

Another limitation of this study is that it relied on

self-report instruments. Thus, the results are dependent on

how the adolescents perceive their own characteristics.

Research that obtains information from others would add a more

complete understanding of the characteristics of this

population. Additionally, although the total number of

subjects was adequate, a larger sample of students, especially

females, displaying significant discrepancies in their

abilities would have allowed a more complete analysis of the

differences between the verbally and mathematically gifted

students.

Lastly, the high number of t-tests increases the

likelihood of experiment-wise error. The consistency of

findings on similar themes measured by the four different

instruments, plus the substantial effect sizes for most of the

findings, do add confidence to the overall results, however.

In today's Zeitgeist, much attention is focused on the

diversity of our population to ensure that the sensitivities

of each group are recognized, that their contributions are

appreciated, and that all are encouraged in their efforts to

have productive, and fulfilling lives. It is often assumed,

but inaccurately so, that the intellectually gifted need no

Page 84: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

78

special help—that they have no special needs or problems. It

is true that the struggles of very bright students are not as

apparent as struggles caused by racial differences or

handicapping conditions. They do, however, cause an intense,

inner pain that can hinder achievement. Our intellectually-

gifted youngsters have the capability to make enormous

contributions to the world. Efforts that address their

special needs can benefit them individually and society as a

whole.

REFERENCES

Albert, R. S. (1978). Observations and suggestions regarding giftedness, faunilial influence, and the achievement of eminence. Gifted Child Quarterly. 22. 201-211.

Allport, G. W., Vernon, P. E., & Lindzey, G. (1970). Manual Study of Values. Boston; Houghton Mifflin.

Allport, G. W., Vernon, P. E., & Lindzey, G. (1960). Manual for the study of values; A scale for measuring the dominant interests in personality (3rd ed). Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Arch, E. (1993). Risk-taking; A motivational basis for sex differences. Psychological Reports. 73, 3-11.

Archer, J., & Lloyd, B. (1985). Sex and gender. New York; Cambridge University Press.

Astin, H. S. (1974). Sex differences in mathematical and scientific precocity. In J. C. Stanley, D. P. Keating, & L. H. Fox (Eds.), Mathematical talent; Discovery, description, and development (pp. 70-86). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Badgett, J. L., Fair, S., & Hunkler, R. F. (1974). The authoritarianism exhibited by intelligent men and women. Journal of College Student Personnel. 15. 509-514.

Benbow, C. P., & Minor, L. L. (1990). Cognitive profiles of verbally and mathematically precocious students: Implications for identification of the gifted. Gifted Child Quarterly. 34. 21-26.

Page 85: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

79

Berndtf D. J., Kaiser, C. F., & van Aalst, F. (1982). Depression and self-actualization in gifted adolescents. Journal of Clinical Psvcholoav. 38. 142-150.

Bernstein, G. A., Garfinkel, B. D., & Hoberman, H. M. (1989). Self-reported anxiety in adolescents. American Journal of Psychiatry. 146. 384-386.

Block, J. H. (1983). Differential premises arising from differential socialization of the sexes: Some conjectures. Child Development. 54. 1335-1354.

Brandwein, P. F. (1955). The gifted student as future scientist. New York; Harcourt, Brace.

Braungart, R. G. (1975). Youth and social movements. In S. E. Dragastin & G. H. Elder, Jr. (Eds.), Adolescence in the Life Cycle, (pp. 255-289). New York: Wiley.

Brode, D. S. (1980). Group dynamics with gifted adolescents. G/C/T. 15, 60-62.

Burke, R. J., & Weir, T. (1978). Sex differences in adolescent life stress, social support, and well-being. Journal of Psychology. 98. 277-288.

Carter, T. (1958). The play problems of gifted children. School Sociology. 86. 224-225.

Campbell, D. K. (1977). Manual for the SVIB-SCII (2nd ed.). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (Rev. ed.). New York: Academic Press.

Cohen, P., Cohen, J., Kasen, S., Velez, C. N., Hartmart, C., Johnson, J., Rojaz, M., Brook, J., & Streuning, E. L. (1993). An epidemiological study in late childhood and adolescence—I. age- and gender-specific prevalence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psvchiatiry. 34. 851-867.

Cox, R. L. (1981). Personal, physical, and feimily traits of gifted children. In B. S. Miller & M. Price (Eds.), The gifted child, the fcunilv and the community (pp. 107-113). New York; Walker & Company.

Dabrowski, K. (1972). Psvchoneurosis is not an illness. London: Gryf.

Page 86: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

80

Dark, V. J., & Benbow, C. P. (1990). Enhanced problem translation and short-term memory: Components of mathematical talent. Journal of Educational Psvcholoav. 82, 420-429.

Davids, A. (1966). Psychological characteristics of high school male and female potential scientists in comparison with academic underachievers. Psvcholoav in the Schools. 3, 79-87.

D'Heurle, A., Mellinger, J. C., & Haggard, E. A. (1959). Personality, intellectual, and achievement patterns in gifted children. Psychological Monographs; General and Applied. 73.(13), 1-28.

Eme, R. F. (1979). Sex differences in childhood psychopathology: A review. Psvchological Bulletin. 86. 574-595.

Ferguson, L. R., & Maccoby, E. E. (1966). Interpersonal correlates of differential abilitities. Child Development. 37. 549-571.

Fox, L. H. (1976). The values of gifted youth. In D. P. Keating (Ed.), Intellectual talent; Research and development (pp 273-284). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Fox, L. H. (1977). Sex differences; Implications for program planning for the academically gifted. In J. C. Stanley, S. C. George, & C. H. Solano (Eds.), The gifted and the creative; A fiftv-vear perspective (pp. 113-138). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Fox, L. H., & Denhcun, S. A. (1974). Values and career interests of mathematically and scientifically precocious youth. In J. C. Stanley, K. P. Keating, & L. H. Fox (Eds.), Mathematical talent; Discovery, description and development (pp. 140-175). Baltimore; Johns Hopkins University Press.

Gottsdanker, J. S. (1968). Intellectual interest patterns of gifted college students. Educational and Psychological Measurement. 28, 361-366.

Gough, H. G., & Heilbrun, A. B. (1983). The Adjective Check List Manual. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Greenlaw, M. J., & Mcintosh, M. E. (1988). Educating the gifted; A source book. Chicago: American Library Association.

Page 87: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

81

Grinder, R. E. (1985). The gifted in our midst: By their divine deeds, neuroses, and mental test scores we have known them. In F. D. Horowitz & M. O'Brien (Eds.), The gifted and talented; Developmental perspectives (pp. 5-35). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Haier, R. J., & Denham, S. A. (1976). A summary profile of the nonintellectual correlates of mathematical precocity in boys and girls. In D. P. Keating (Ed.), Intellectual talent; Research and development (pp. 225-241). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Haier, R. J., & Solano, C. H. (1976). Educators' stereotypes of mathematically gifted boys. In D. P. Keating (Ed.), Intellectual talent; Research and development (pp. 215-222). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Halpin, G., Payne, D., & Ellett, C. (1975). Life history antecedents of current personality traits of gifted adolescents. Measurement and Evaluation in Guidance. 8(1), 29-36.

Harmon, L. W., Hansen, J. C., Borgen, F. H., & Hammer, A. L. (1994). Applications and technical guide for the Strong Interest Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Hildreth, G. (1938). Characteristics of young gifted children. Journal of Genetic Psvchology. 53. 287-311.

Holland, J. L. (1970). The Self-Directed Search. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Holland, J. L. (1975). Manual for the Vocational Preference Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Hollinger, C. L. (1983). Counseling the gifted and talented female adolescent: The relationship between social self-esteem and traits of instrumentality and expressiveness. Gifted Child Quarterly, 27# 157-161.

Hollingworth, L. S. (1926). Gifted children; Their nature and nurture. New York: Macmillan.

Hollingworth, L. S. (1942). Children above 180 10 Stanford Binet; Origin and development. Yonkers, NY: World Book.

Hyde, J. S. (1990). Meta-analysis and the psychology of gender differences. Signs. 16. 56-73.

Page 88: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

82

Jacklin, C. N. (Ed.). (1992). The psvcholoctv of gender. Vol. III. New York: New York University Press.

Janos, P. M., Fung, H. C., & Robinson, H. M. (1985). Self-concept, self-esteem, and peer relations among gifted children who feel "different." Gifted Child Quarterly. 29, 78-82.

Jensen, M. B. (1994). Psychological well-being of intellectually precocious youth and peers at commensurate levels of socioeconomic status. Unpublished master's thesis, Iowa State University, Ames, lA.

Kelly, K. R., & Colangelo, N. (1984). Academic and social self-concepts of gifted, general, and special students. Exceptional children. 50. 551-554.

Kerr, B. A. (1991). A handbook for counseling the gifted and talented. Alexandria, VA: American Counseling Association.

Killian, J. (1983). Personality characteristics of intellectually gifted secondary students. Roeper Review, 5, 39-42.

Kline, B. E., & Meckstroth, E. A. (1985). Understanding and encouraging the exceptionally gifted. Roeper Review. 24-30.

Lapan, R. T., Boggs, K. R., & Morrill, W. H. (1989). Self-efficacy as a mediator of investigative and realistic General Occupational Themes on the Strong-Campbell Interest Inventory. Journal of Counseling Psvchology. 36. 176-182.

Lehman, E. B., & Erdwins, C. J. (1981). The social and emotional adjustment of young, intellectually-gifted children. Gifted Child Quarterly. 25. 134-137.

Liddle, G. (1958). Overlap among desirable and undersirable characteristics in gifted children. Journal of Educational Psychology. 49, 219-223.

Loeb, R. C., & Jay, G. (1987). Self-concept in gifted children: Differential impact in boys and girls. Gifted Child Quarterly. 31, 9-13.

Lovecky, D. V. (1992). The exceptionally gifted child. Understanding our Gifted. ̂ (3), 7-9.

Page 89: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

83

Lovecky, D. V. (1993). The quest for meaning: Counseling issues with gifted children and adolescents. In L. K. Silverman (Ed.), Counaeling the gifted and talented (pp. 29-50). Denver: Love Publishing.

Lubinski^ D.^ & Benbow, C. P. (1992). Gender differences in abilities and preferences among the gifted: Implications for the math-science pipeline. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 61-66.

Lubinski, D., Benbow, C. P., & Sanders, C. E. (1993). Reconceptualizing gender differences in achievement eunong the gifted. In K. A. Heller, F. J. Monks, & A. H. Passow (Eds.), International handbook of research and development of giftedness and talent (pp. 693-707). London; Pergeimon Press.

Ludwig, G., & Cullinan, D. (1984). Behavior problems of gifted and nongifted elementary school girls and boys. Gifted Child Quarterly. 28, 37-39.

Mason, E., Adams, H., & Blood, D. (1968). Further study of personality characteristics of bright college freshmen. Psychological Reports, 23. 395-400.

Maccoby, E., & Rau, L. (1963). Differential cognitive abilities (U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, Cooperative Research Project No. 1040). Stanford, CA: Stanford University.

Mayer, J. D., Caruso, D. R., Zigler, E., & Dreyden, J. I. (1987). Intelligence and intelligence-related personality traits. Intelligence, 13,, 119-133.

McCarthy, S. V. (1975). Differential V-Q ability; Twenty years later. Review of Educational Research. 45. 263-282.

McCurdy, H. G. (1957). The childhood pattern of genius. Journal of Elisha Mitchell Science Society. 73. 448-462.

McGinn, P. V. (1976). Verbally gifted youth: Selection and description. In D. P. Keating (Ed.), Intellectual talent: Research and development (pp. 160-182). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Miles, C. C. (1954). Gifted children. In L. Carmichael (Ed.), Manual of child psychology (pp. 984-1008). New York; Wiley.

Myers, I. B. (1962). The Mvers-Briggs Type Indicator Manual. Princeton: Educational Testing Service.

Page 90: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

84

Psiyns f D • ^ • f Hdlpxii f W • G * f & Ellett f C» D« (X973)* Personality trait characteristics of differentially gifted students. Psvcholoav in the Schools. 10. 189-195.

Piechowski, M. M. (1991). Emotional development and emotional giftedness. In N. Colangelo & G. A. Davis (Eds.), Handbook of gifted education (pp. 285-307). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Purkey, W. W. (1966). Measured and professed personality characteristics of gifted high school students and an analysis of their congruence. Journal of Educational Research. 60. 99-103.

Roe, A. (1952). A psychologist excunines 64 eminent scientists. Scientific American. 182(5), 21-25.

Ross, A., & Parker, M. (1980). Academic and social self concepts of the academically gifted. Exceptional Children. 42(2), 6-10.

Sanders, E. M., Mefferd, R. B., Jr., & Bown, 0. H. (1960). Verbal-quantitative ability and certain personality and metabolic characteristics of male college students. Educational and Psvchological Measurement. 20. 491-503.

Shade, R. (1991). Verbal humor in gifted students and students in the general population; A comparison of spontaneous mirth and comprehension. Journal for the Education of the Gifted. 14. 134-150.

Sheldon, P. M. (1959). Isolation as a characteristic of highly gifted children. Journal of Educational Sociology. 32, 215-222.

Silverman, L. K. (1986). Parenting young gifted children. Journal of Children in Contemporary Society. 18. 73-87.

Silverman, L. K. (1993). A developmental model for counseling the gifted. In L. K. Silverman (Ed.), Counseling the gifted and talented (pp. 51-78). Denver; Love Publishing.

Smith, D. C. (1962). Personal and social adjustment of gifted adolescents. Research Monograph No. 4. Washington DC; National Education Association, Council for Exceptional Children.

Southern, M. L., & Plant, W. T. (1968). Personality characteristics of very bright adults. Journal of Social Psychology. 75, 119-126.

Page 91: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

85

Subotnik, R./ Summers, E., Kassan, L., & Wasser, A. (1993). Genius revisited; Hiah 10 children grown up. Norwood, NJ; Ablex Publishing.

Swiatek, M. A. (1993). Academic and psychosocial perspectives on giftedness during adolescence. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation. Iowa State University, Ames, lA.

Terman, L. M. (1925). Genetic studies of genius; Mental and physical traits of a thousand gifted children. Stanford, CA; Stanford University Press.

Tellegen, A., & Waller, N. G. (1982). Exploring personality through test construction; Development of the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. Unpublished manuscript.

Thorndike, E. L. (1911). Individuality. New York: Houghton-Mifflin.

Viernstein, M. C., McGinn, P. V., & Hogan, R. (1977). The personality correlates of differential verbal and mathematical ability in talented adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 6, 169-178.

Wall, W. D. (1960). Highly intelligent children; Part I—the psychology of the gifted. Educational Research. 2., 101-110.

Warren, J. R., & Heist, P. A. (I960). Personality attributes of gifted college students. Science. 132. 330-337.

Werner, E. E., & Bachtold, L. M. (1969). Personality factors of gifted boys and girls in middle childhood and adolescence. Psychology in the schools. 6, 177-182.

Whitmore, J. R. (1980). Giftedness. conflict, and underachievement. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Page 92: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

•— Primarily • Primarily •— Primarily • Primarily

mathematically verbally gifted mathematically verbally gifted

gifted (males) (males) gifted (females)

(females)

56

54

52

50 V)

I 48 S

46

• / 44

4 2

40 (D "D

0) O C o

Q) O 8, 0)

o c o o

•D c IIJ

D) O)

Figure 1. Adjective Check List. T Scores: M=50; SD=10.

Page 93: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

•— Primarily • Primarily • Primarily • Primarily

mathematically verbally gifted mathematically verbally gifted

gifted (males) (males) gifted (females) (females)

CO

Figure 2. Multridimensional Personality Questionnaire. Ravj Scores. Scores in parentheses are mean scores for normative group. F=females; M=males

Page 94: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

•— — Primarily • Primarily • Primarily • Primarily

mathematically verbally gifted mathematicaily verbally gifted

gifted (males) (males) gifted (females) (females)

58

56

54

52

50

48

46

44

42 N.

40

38

36

Enterprising Conventional Investigative Artistic Social Realistic

Figure 3. General Occupational Themes. T Scores: M=50; SD=10.

Page 95: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

•— Primarily • Primarily • Primarily • Primarily

mathematically verbally gifted mathematically verbally gifted

gifted (males) (males) gifted (females) (females)

58

56

54

52

50

48

46

44

42

40

Religion Music Athletics Adventure A r t Nature

Figure 4. Basic Tnterest Scales. T Scores: M = 50; SD=10.

Page 96: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

•— — Primarily • Primarily • Primarily • Primarily

mathematically verbally gifted mathematically verbally gifted

gifted (males) (males) gifted (females) (females)

48

46

N. 44

42

40

38

36

34

32

30

Religious Political Social Aesthetic Economical Theoretical

o

Figure 5. Study of Values. Scales are ipsative; scores combined for an individual for six scales must equal 240.

Page 97: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

91

GENERAL SUMMARY

Precocious people are different enough from their peers

to attract attention. Historically, extreme intelligence has

been associated with mental illness and other undesirable

traits (Grinder, 1985; Lombroso, 1891). Although these myths

have been refuted by the work of Terman (1925), Hollingworth

(1926), and others, highly-intelligent individuals still are

not well understood. In this dissertation, the research

literature pertaining to the personalities of highly-

intelligent people is reviewed. In addition, an empirical

study is presented that examined the questions of whether

characteristics of intellectually-gifted people are related to

their domain of giftedness, and if so, does gender interact

with domain to influence traits of gifted individuals?

Subjects of the research were junior high students who

had been identified as highly gifted either in mathematics or

in verbal ability. This distinction allowed personality

comparisons using the Adjective Check List, Study of Values,

Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire, and Strong

Interest Inventory. Comparisons were done separately by

gender.

When compared to the general population, this group of

intellectually gifted adolescents was found to be driven by a

compelling need for knowledge and understanding of the world

around them. In spite a sense of alienation they felt, they

Page 98: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

92

were confident of themselves. They made up their own minds

about issues and were not especially respectful of authority.

Some gender differences emerged in this group. The

gifted males were more driven by their theoretical interests.

The females, while also having high theoretical interests,

demonstrated much more interest in social activities and

aesthetic pursuits. The gifted girls also placed more value

on religious activities.

There were also differences related to talent domain that

can be summed up saying that the verbally-gifted adolescents

were less social and appeared to dislike the constraints of

limits imposed by society or even themselves. Thus, they

appear to be at somewhat more risk for social or psychological

distress.

Young people with high intellectual abilities are those

with the greatest potential to make contributions to benefit

future generations. These findings that add to our

understanding of this population have implications for gifted

education, which has been given the task of helping highly-

intelligent young people develop their potential, enabling

them to contribute to society and lead lives that are

satisfying and fulfilling.

Page 99: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

93

ADDITIONAL REFERENCES

Benbow, C. P., & Minor, L. L. (1990). Cognitive profiles of verbally and mathematically precocious students: Implications for identification of the gifted. Gifted Child Quarterly. 34, 21-26.

Grinder, R. E. (1985). The gifted in our midst: By their divine deeds, neuroses, and mental test scores we have known them. In F. D. Horowitz & M. O'Brien (Eds.), The gifted and talented; Developmental perspectives (pp. 5-35). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Hollingworth, L. W. (1926). Gifted children; Their nature and nurture. New York: Macmillan.

Lombroso, C. (1891). The man of genius. London: Scott.

Terman, L. M. (1925). Genetic studies of genius; Mental and physical traits of a thousand gifted children. Stanford, CA; Stanford University Press.

Page 100: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

94

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

I would like to give heartfelt thanks to all who have

offered assistance and support to allow me to complete this

dissertation. Specifically, I would like to thank the members

of my committee. Dr. Lynn Glass, Dr. Dan Russell, and Dr.

David Lubinski for their encouragement and suggestions.

Special thanks are extended to Dr. Norman Scott, who has been

generous with his time, serving as both a committee member and

as my co-major professor, and to Dr. Camilla Persson Benbow,

whose patience and encouragement in helping me develop and

write this dissertation are greatly appreciated.

Additionally, I will always appreciate the support of my

children, John and Carol Kosmicke, who told me I could do it.

Page 101: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

95

APPENDIX

DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS FOR THE SCALES USED IN THIS STUDY

Scales N Mean SD

Study of Values*

Theoretical Normative Normative Males Females Primarily Primarily Primarily Primarily

Economic Normative Normative Males Females Primarily Primarily Primarily Primarily

Aesthetic Normative Normative Males Females Primarily Primarily Primarily Primarily

Social Normative Normative Males Females Primarily Primarily Primarily Primarily

Males Females

Mathematical—Males Mathematical—Females Verbal—Males Verbal—Females

Males Females

Mathematical—Males Mathematical—Females Verbal—Males Verbal—Females

Males Females

Mathematical—Males Mathematical—Females Verbal—Males Verbal—Females

Males Females

Mathematical—Males Mathematical—Females Verbal—Males Verbal—Females

1838 44.59 6.48 2306 37.80 6.80 311 47.59 7.06 219 41.06 7.19 108 47.71 6.89 36 42.58 8.94 59 46.82 6.12 88 39.40 6.61

1838 42.41 6.90 2306 37.70 6.15 311 42.97 7.36 219 36.68 7.22 108 43.13 7.21 36 39.14 7.29 59 42.17 7.26 88 34.42 7.47

1838 35.00 7.44 2306 38.02 7.13 311 37.61 8.11 219 43.72 7.73 108 35.41 6.85 36 42.76 6.28 59 41.55 8.74 88 46.02 7.55

1838 37.35 6.39 2306 43.47 7.00 311 35.90 7.00 219 43.11 7.78 108 36.61 6.32 36 43.22 7.00 59 35.75 6.97 88 43.93 8.40

' Scales are ipsative; Scores combined for an individual for six scales must equal 240.

Page 102: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

96

Scales N Mean SD

Study of Values*

Political Normative Normative Males Females Primarily Primarily Primarily Primarily

Religious Normative Normative Males Females Primarily Primarily Primarily Primarily

Males Females

Mathematical—Males Mathematical—Females Verbal—Males Verbal—Females

Males Females

Mathematical—Males Mathematical—Females Verbal—Males Verbal—Females

Adjective Check List*"

Autonomy Normative Normative Males Females Primarily Primarily Primarily Primarily

Aggression Normative Normative Males Females Primarily Primarily Primarily Primarily

Males Females

Mathematical—Males Mathematical—Females Verbal—Males Verbal—Females

Males Females

Mathematical—Males Mathematical—Females Verbal—Males Verbal—Females

1838 42.94 5.79 2306 39.40 6.04 311 43.48 6.17 219 39.28 6.95 108 43.07 6.47 36 39.08 7.07 59 42.92 5.96 88 38.89 7.20

1838 37.09 7.55 2306 43.11 8.29 311 32.45 10.88 219 36.10 9.93 108 34.07 11.62 36 33.21 9.60 59 30.80 11.16 88 37.34 9.69

128 52.34 9.89 126 51.87 9.59 321 52.47 8.90 224 53.76 9.13 109 50.18 7.67 37 52.87 8.94 62 53.06 9.21 92 54.80 9.82

128 52.00 10.54 126 52.13 9.64 321 54.17 9.48 224 53.14 9.97 109 52.86 8.70 37 51.41 9.98 62 55.53 9.84 92 53.26 10.06

" Scores are ipsative; Scores individual must equal 240. T scores: M = 50; SD = 10.

combined for six scales for an

Page 103: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

97

Scales N Mean SD

Adjective Check List'

Change Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

Deference Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

Order Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

Self-Confidence Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

128 53 .46 9. 45 126 54 .40 9. 40 321 52 .10 9. 36 224 52 .57 10. 32 109 51 .32 8. 70 37 53 .32 10. 39 62 52 .95 9. 89 92 53 .35 10. 44

128 46 .84 10. 34 126 47 .75 9. 70 321 44 .55 9. 66 224 44 .37 10. 59 109 46 .76 8. 61 37 46 .08 10. 70 62 43 .50 9. 99 92 43 .39 10. 58

128 44 .40 10. 05 126 43 .17 9. 09 321 45 .20 9. 37 224 46 .26 9. 84 109 46 .23 9. 77 37 47 .03 10. 78 62 44 .11 10. 10 92 45 .83 10. 56

128 46 .70 10. 28 126 45 .87 8. 56 321 50 .96 9. 66 224 51 .14 10. 46 109 50 .76 10. 56 37 51 .30 9. 71 62 50 .21 8. 66 92 50 .10 11. 92

T scores; M = 50; SD = 10

Page 104: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

98

Scales N Mean SD

Adjective Check List"

Self-Control Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

Affiliation Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

Endurance Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire

Weil-Being Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

128 45 .48 9. 65 126 45 .25 10. 83 321 43 .28 10. 19 224 44 .19 11. 06 109 45 .15 9. 66 37 44 .43 10. 60 62 41 .77 11. 50 92 43 .88 11. 79

128 46 .66 8. 86 126 49 .07 9. 38 321 47 .40 9. 80 224 48 .54 11. 28 109 48 .34 9. 77 37 49 .43 9. 50 62 46 .71 9. 47 92 48 .43 11. 66

128 43 .88 9. 84 126 42 .40 8. 83 321 45 .96 9. 21 224 47 .67 8. 54 109 47 .22 9. 75 37 48 .62 8. 57 62 44 .73 9. 69 92 47 .52 9. 34

300 19 .1 5. 0 500 18 .9 5. 1 206 16 .49 5. 64 127 17 .80 5. 77 67 16 .66 5. 48 16 17 .06 6. 04 44 15 .98 5. 99 54 18 .22 5. 57

" T scores: M = 50; SD = 10

Page 105: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

99

Scales N Mean SD

Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire

Social Potency Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

Achievement Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

Social Closeness Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

Stress Reaction Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

Alienation Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

300 13 .6 6 .0 500 11 .2 6 .3 206 12 .46 6 .07 127 13 .38 6 .31 67 11 .01 5 .85 16 11 .69 7 .30 44 12 .52 5 .72 54 13 .31 6 .50

300 11 .9 4 .8 500 11 .8 4 .7 206 11 .23 4 .26 127 12 .09 4 .80 67 11 .73 4 .07 16 12 .25 5 .42 44 11 .18 4 .96 54 12 .31 4 .71

300 15 .2 4 .7 500 16 .1 4 .5 206 12 .43 4 .71 127 13 .83 5 .26 67 12 .76 4 .54 16 14 .13 5 .74 44 11 .50 5 .01 54 13 .46 4 .63

300 10 .4 6 .5 500 12 .3 6 .5 206 11 .96 6 .11 127 13 .50 6 .16 67 12 .13 5 .93 16 12 .88 6 .17 44 11 .50 5 .01 54 14 .00 6 .09

300 2 .9 3 .7 500 2 .0 2 .7 206 8 .05 5 .13 127 5 .46 4 .25 67 7 .93 5 .23 16 5 .88 4 .88 44 8 .84 5 .41 54 5 .91 4 .13

Page 106: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

100

Scales N Mean SD

Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire

Aggression Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

Control Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

Harm Avoidance Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical-"Femalee Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

Traditionalism Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

Absorption Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

300 7 .0 4 .6 500 4 .4 3 .3 206 10 .98 5 .13 127 6 .01 4 .13 67 9 .67 5 .17 16 5 .13 3 .59 44 11 .73 5 .06 54 5 .41 3 .31

300 14 .1 5 .0 500 14 .3 3 .3 206 11 .93 4 .92 127 12 .47 5 .71 67 12 .90 5 .02 16 12 .50 6 .63 44 10 .98 4 .91 54 12 .91 5 .90

300 13 .1 5 .8 500 17 .0 5 .7 206 10 .64 6 .06 127 13 .54 6 .52 67 12 .22 5 .83 16 14 .06 7 .90 44 9 .43 6 .19 54 14 .48 6 .17

300 14 .5 5 .5 500 13 .0 5 .8 206 14 .87 6 .26 127 15 .37 5 .36 67 17 .33 5 .60 16 14 .63 5 .16 44 13 .75 6 .28 54 15 .67 5 .83

300 19 .6 7 .3 500 21 .4 6 .9 206 17 .51 7 .78 107 21 .65 7 .41 67 16 .70 7 .73 16 19 .38 6 .95 44 19 .34 7 .42 54 22 .50 6 .93

Page 107: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

101

Scales N Mean SD

General Occupational Themes'

Realistic Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

Investigative Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

Artistic Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

Social Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

77 51 .6 10 .3 71 40 .4 9 .4

204 48 .23 9 .17 127 43 .76 8 .50 65 48 .82 9 .23 18 44 .33 8 .57 43 50 .23 10 .63 54 42 .59 7 .37

77 50 .8 10 .4 71 45 .2 10 .2

204 53 .55 8 .11 127 52 .79 8 .92 65 55 .29 7 .10 18 54 .44 9 .01 43 53 .53 8 .38 54 40 .56 8 .52

77 48 .3 10 .7 71 51 .1 10 .5

204 43 .21 10 .58 127 54 .03 9 .25 65 41 .43 9 .54 18 49 .50 10 .71 43 43 .79 11 .25 54 57 .04 8 .20

77 46 .0 10 .6 71 50 .5 9 .9

204 39 .41 10 .11 127 49 .23 10 .56 65 41 .60 10 .18 18 46 .83 11 .56 43 38 .37 10 .52 54 49 .56 10 .78

• T scores: M = 50; SD = 10.

Page 108: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

102

Scales N Mean SD

General Occupational Themes"

Enterprising Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

Conventional Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

Basic Interest Scales'

Nature Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

Adventure Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

77 51 .4 9. 4 71 51 .8 9. 0 204 43 .29 9. 40 127 44 .76 9. 85 65 44 .06 8. 93 18 42 .83 12. 25 43 43 .07 9. 32 54 44 .13 9. 50

77 52 .8 10. 3 71 53 .4 10. 6 204 46 .67 9. 81 127 47 .13 10. 25 65 50 .15 9. 99 18 51 .39 11. 35 43 45 .37 10. 40 54 44 .89 10. 13

300 49 .3 9. 9 300 50 .7 10. 1 204 41 .26 9. 15 127 50 .36 9. 62 65 41 .80 9. 69 18 48 .56 11. 19 43 41 .53 9. 79 54 51 .31 8. 47

300 52 .7 9. 7 300 47 .3 9. 5 204 57 .09 9. 24 127 51 .28 10. 01 65 55 .46 9. 72 18 48 .39 11. 19 43 59 .42 8. 81 54 50 .57 9. 04

• T scores; M = 50; SD = 10.

Page 109: Personality and interests of gifted adolescents

103

Scales N Mean SD

Basic Interest Scales®

Music Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

Art Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

Athletics Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

Religion Normative Males Normative Females Males Females Primarily Mathematical—Males Primarily Mathematical—Females Primarily Verbal—Males Primarily Verbal—Females

300 46 .9 9 .9 300 53 .1 9 .0 204 43 .90 9 .01 127 55 .30 8 .91 65 43 .05 7 .76 18 53 .22 8 .80 43 44 .05 9 .78 54 57 .33 9 .23

300 46 .5 9 .8 300 53 .5 9 .0 204 43 .30 9 .61 127 54 .01 10 .03 65 41 .66 9 .09 18 50 .67 11 .82 43 44 .19 9 .91 54 57 .06 8 .68

300 53 .1 9 .8 300 46 .9 9 .0 204 48 .77 9 .24 127 45 .90 8 .16 65 49 .14 9 .51 18 45 .67 8 .42 43 46 .23 9 .37 54 44 .07 8 .25

300 49 .6 9 .9 300 50 .4 10 .1 204 43 .09 9 .94 127 48 .00 10 .21 65 45 .31 10 .45 18 46 .17 8 .56 43 42 .56 10 .03 54 48 .54 10 .04

T Scores; M = 50; SD = 10


Recommended