+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Psychobiological Model of Temperament and Character (1)

Psychobiological Model of Temperament and Character (1)

Date post: 07-Aug-2018
Category:
Upload: stojan-bajraktarov
View: 220 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend

of 7

Transcript
  • 8/20/2019 Psychobiological Model of Temperament and Character (1)

    1/16

    A Psychobiological Model   of Temperamentand CharacterC.   Robert Cloninger,  MD;  Dragan  M.  Svrakic,  MD,  PhD;   Thomas   R. Przybeck,  PhD

    Inthis  study,   we  describe   a psychobiological  model  of  the  structure   and   development   of

    personality   that  accounts   for dimensions of both temperament  and   character.  Previous   re-search  has confirmed  four  dimensions  of temperament:  novelty  seeking,  harm   avoidance,reward  dependence,   and  persistence, which  are independently heritable,   manifest  early   in

    life, and  involve preconceptual biases   in perceptual memory and habit formation. For the first time,we  describe   three  dimensions  of character   that  mature   in  adulthood   and   influence personal  andsocial effectiveness by insight learning about self-concepts. Self-concepts vary according to the  extentto which   a person  identifies   the  self   as  (1)  an  autonomous  individual,   (2)  an integral  part  of hu-

    manity, and (3)  an integral part of the  universe   as   a whole.  Each aspect of self-concept correspondsto   one   of   three character   dimensions   called  self-directedness,   cooperativeness,   and   self-transcendence,   respectively.   We  also   describe   the   conceptual  background   and   development  of   aself-report  measure of these   dimensions,   the  Temperament  and  Character  Inventory.  Data on 300individuals from the general population support the  reliability and structure of these seven person-ality  dimensions.   We  discuss   the  implications   for   studies  of information   processing,   inheritance,

    development,  diagnosis,  and  treatment.   (Arch  Gen Psychiatry.   1993;50:975-990)Several lines of research are converging tofacilitate the integration of categorical anddimensional  methods  for diagnosing per¬sonality disorder. First, explicit diagnosticcriteria  were developed   to   define   tradi¬tional categories of personality disorders,1and structured interviews were developedthat make such diagnoses  reliably.2·3   Sec¬ond, self-report  scales for  rating quantita¬tive dimensions of personality were devel¬oped using factor analysis of traits observedin   individuals   with  personality   disor¬ders4"6 or in the general population'^; thesewere shown to  be reliable and to  correlate

    highly   with  one   another.3·10  Third,   self-reported dimensional measures were shownto explain much of the reliable variance  ininterview diagnoses  of personality   disor¬ders11   and   to  agree closely with  indepen-

    dent  reports of spouses  and  other   collat¬eral  informants.10

    Despite  this  progress   in   assessmentmethods,  no consensus  has  been reachedon  the  number  or content of  the dimen¬sions needed   to   describe  personality   dis¬order.5·12·13 Five factors, plus or minus two,account   for most  variation   in personalitybetween individuals   in  the general popu¬lation.12·13 When  observer-rated or   self-

    reported  behavior  is  factor analyzed,   twofactors  like neuroticism   (vs   stability)   andextraversion   (vs  introversion)   are  consis¬

    tently described. There is   less  consistencyin the  content of the third  factor; Eysenckand Eysenck7 described tough-mindedness;Tellegen,9 constraint; and Costa and McCrae,10openness to experience.  In a popular five-factor model, two additional factors are con¬scientiousness  and agreeableness,  but   theoptimal rotation and  content of alternativefive-factor models remain

     subjects ofactive-

    From   the Department of Psychiatry,  Washington  University  School of  Medicine,St  Louis,   Mo.

    wnloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Virginia User on 07/27/2012

  • 8/20/2019 Psychobiological Model of Temperament and Character (1)

    2/16

    debate.1014 Furthermore,   the   five-factor model  does not capture somedomains ofpersonality relevant to per¬sonality   disorders,   such   as   indi¬vidual  autonomy,   traditional moral

    values,   and   other aspects of  matu¬rity  and   self-actualization describedin humanistic and transpersonal psy¬

    chology.13   Studies   of natural   lan¬guage provide evidence of seven  d i¬mensions of personality, including twoself-evaluative  dimensions for  goodand   bad   self-concepts   that   arelabeled   positive   and   negativevalence.12

    Nevertheless,   as the number ofmeasured   factors   increases   in  avail¬able instruments, the cumulative pro¬portion of shared   variance   is likelyto increase between alternative mod¬

    els.   In other words, what   is left outof one  model  can be  added   until   acomprehensive   set   is  accumulated.However,   factor   analysis  of   indi¬vidual case descriptions can only de¬termine   the   minimum   number   ofmeasured dimensions, and  cannot de¬compose their underlying causal struc¬ture.   Extrastatistical   information   isneeded to specify the structure of theunderlying biologic  and social vari¬ability   in personality traits.   In other

    words,   descriptive   data about   indi¬vidual behavior are not sufficient to per¬mit any   strong preference  among   al¬ternative   ways   of  summarizingpersonality  traits. Advocates of a par¬ticular model derived by factor analy¬sis   must ultimately base   their pref¬erence on authority or tradition if theyeschew external etiologic criteria.8 Anexample of utmost importance  in theassessment of personality disorder isthe content of the  factor called neu-

    roticism. According to the  authors ofa popular  five-factor  inventory, neu-roticism measures adjustment  com¬pared with emotional instability andidentifies individuals who are proneto psychological distress, unrealisticideas, excessive cravings or urges, andmaladaptive coping responses.15 ft isa clinically heterogeneous  compos¬ite  of anxiety,  hostility,   depression,self-consciousness, impulsiveness, and

    general emotional vulnerability.

     High

    neuroticism scores are frequent in in¬dividuals   with personality   disor¬der,11 but are not diagnostically  spe¬cific;   many   psychiatric   patientswithout personality disorder also havehigh neuroticism scores and some in¬dividuals with high neuroticism scoreshave  no psychiatric   disorder.13   Ac¬

    cordingly,   the  use of  the   five-factormodel for clinical diagnosis has beencriticized   in part  because   the   con¬tent of  its  individual  factors is  clini¬

    cally  heterogeneous.12In addition, the  content of  fac¬

    tors defined by  factor analysis of in¬dividual  case descriptions is also etio-logically heterogeneous. Gray16·17 usedobservations about the  effects of an-

    tianxiety drugs on personality to   ar¬gue   that   the   behavioral   factors  of

    neuroticism   and   extraversion-introversion do not correspond to theirunderlying  biologic   variation.   Heshowed that drugs that reduce scoreson measures of neuroticism,  such asalcohol   and   benzodiazepines,   alsoconsistently   reduce   scores  on mea¬sures of introversion,  suggesting thatthese   dimensions   are  not etiologi-cally independent, but  rather sharebiological  determinants.   Likewise,Cloninger1819  showed that  the  phe-notypic structure of personality maydiffer   from   the   underlying  bioge-netic structure because the observedbehavioral   variation   is  the  result ofthe   interaction of  genetic  and  envi¬ronmental  influences.  For example,extraversion  and  tough-mindednessare composed  of  elements   that   aregenetically   independent  but   sharecommon  environmental  factors.20"22Such  experimental   information  onetiologic   factors   helps   to   chooseamong   an  infinite   set  of alternativemodels of personality  structure.

    To   test hypotheses  about   thecausal structure of personality,   a gen¬eral  psychobiological  model of per¬sonality   was developed   and   de¬scribed by Cloninger18 in two stages.The  model was initially  based on   asynthesis  of information from   twinand family studies, studies of longi¬tudinal development, neuropharma-

    cologic and neurobehavioral studies

    of learning in humans and other ani¬mals,   as well   as psychometric   stud¬ies of personality  in  individuals andin   twin pairs.18  The   original  modeldescribed   three   dimensions of per¬sonality that were postulated  to   begenetically  independent  of one   an¬other. The dimensions were called nov¬

    elty seeking, harm avoidance, and   re¬ward dependence, and were measuredwith   a  100-item self-report inventorycalled the  Trimensional  PersonalityQuestionnaire  (TPQ).23  Recently   themodel was extended to measure sevendimensions of personality  with   theaddition of measures of persistence, self-directedness,   cooperativeness,   andself-transcendence.   This   extension   isbased  on   a synthesis of informationabout social and cognitive  develop¬ment  and descriptions of personalitydevelopment   in   humanistic   andtranspersonal psychology. This seven-factor model supersedes models withfewer  factors and is  formulated to al¬low differential diagnosis of personal¬ity  disorder  subtypes   from   one   an¬other   and   from  other  psychiatricdisorders.

    The model was  extended to  bemore comprehensive   and   to   im¬prove the diagnosis of personality dis¬order. We observed that the originalthree dimensions distinguished amongsubtypes of personality disorders, butdid not consistently differentiate in¬dividuals with personality disordersor poor social adjustment from otherwell-adapted  individuals   with   ex¬treme personality profiles.24 Further¬more,   coverage   of   the personalitydomain was incomplete with  a three-dimensional  model:   some   DSM-lll-R personality   disorders   such   as

    paranoid and   schizotypal  personal¬ity had been neglected, and  some per¬sonality factors measured  in other di¬mensional models   could   not   be

    explained by the TPQ.   In   a compan¬ion  article25  we show that   the   pres¬ence of an interview diagnosis of per¬sonality disorder by DSM-U1-R criteriais consistently predicted by low scoreson two personality dimensions: self-directedness and cooperativeness. De¬pending on  these scores,   the risk of

    wnloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Virginia User on 07/27/2012

  • 8/20/2019 Psychobiological Model of Temperament and Character (1)

    3/16

    personality   disorder   in   a  clinicalsample varied from  11%  to 94%,  sothat   the  relationship   to diagnosis  isstrong.   In addition, the other five di¬mensions permit differential subtyp-ing  of all   the   individual  DSM-III-Rpersonality  disorder categories.

    Next, we briefly summarize the

    development of the model and  thendescribe the structure and content ofthe factors  to inform clinicians abouttheir  practical   clinical   use. Samplequestions are provided.

    MATERIALS   AND METHODS

    Development of  thePsychobiological   Model

    Personality has  been defined   as "the

    dynamic organization  within the  in¬dividual of those psychophysical sys¬tems that determine  his  unique   ad¬

     justments  to   his   environment."26

    Learning  has  been  likewise   definedas "the organization of behavior   as   aresult  of individual  experience."27Therefore, differences between indi¬viduals   in   the  adaptive   systems   in¬volved   in the  reception,   processing,and storing of information about  ex¬perience   define   personality in   gen¬eral.   Two types of memory systemshave   been  distinguished   in   pri¬mates.28"30 Conscious experiences arerepresented as words, images, or sym¬bols about facts and   events that have

    explicit meanings and  functional   re¬lations with one another that we can

    consciously  retrieve,   declare   ver¬bally, and  act on intentionally.  In con¬trast,   unconscious, implicit,  or pro¬cedural memories involve presemanticperceptual  processing  that  encodesconcrete visuospatial   structural   in¬formation and  affective valence; such

    perceptual processing can operate in¬dependent of abstract conceptual, in¬tentional,   or   declarative   pro¬cesses.31·32  For example,  individualswith amnesia who cannot recognizeor recall prior exposure to particularpairs   of words   may  have   long-lasting   affective   responses   and   im¬provement  of their  ability   to   com¬

    plete  the   word   from

     partial  letter

    fragments.33"33  In other words,  con¬scious  memories   are concept-basedwhereas   unconscious memories are

    percept-driven.Hippocampal   processing   and

    long-term storage  in association cor¬tex appear  to  be  essential   for   con¬solidation of explicit memories that

    can be intentionally retrieved; in con¬trast hippocampal processing is notrequired   for   retention   of  implicitmemories that are unintentionally  re¬trieved without any conscious recallof the original experiential events.31·32·36Lesion studies   in humans and  other

    primates   show   that   conceptual   in¬formation is processed and stored ina cortico-limbo-diencephalic  systemthat includes   the  higher order  sen¬sory areas of the cortex, the entorhi-

    nal cortex, the amygdala, hippocam¬pal   formation,   the  medial   thalamicnuclei, ventromedial prefrontal   cor¬tex,  and the basal  forebrain.   In con¬

    trast, perceptual memories underly¬ing unconscious habits are stored   ina  cortico-striatal   system,  which   in¬cludes the sensory cortical areas, andthe caudate and putamen.28·30 An  ex¬ample  of the   anatomical  separationof these two memory systems is that

    monkeys  with combined amygdalo-

    hippocampal removal show severe im¬pairments in conceptual memory tasks(such   as visual recognition) after de¬lays of only  a  minute, but they learnperceptual memory   tasks   (such   asconcurrent   visual   discrimination

    habits)   as quickly   as   normalanimals.28·37

    Associative learning, such  as op¬érant conditioning of habits  and  af¬fective responses, requires direct per¬ceptual experience of the   events butdoes not involve conscious recall or

    recognition of the contingencies thatmodify behavior.16   In contrast,  con¬ceptual learning is conscious and  ab¬stractly symbolic,   as is verbal  learn¬ing in which symbolic communicationcan alter expectancies about the  en¬vironment and  behavior.38

    Prior personality models   failedto distinguish  between  distinct   as¬pects of memory because they werederived

     by factor

     analysis of  behav-

    ior,  not by  consideration of  its  un¬derlying biologic   and   social   deter¬minants.   In   this   psychobiologicalmodel,   four dimensions of person¬ality  involve  automatic,  preconcep-tual responses to perceptual stimuli,presumably   reflecting  heritable   bi¬ases in information processing by the

    perceptual memory system. These fourdimensions will be referred to   as tem¬perament   factors,   in   contrast   to   thethree factors that appear to be concept-based.  The three personality   factorsbased   on   differences   in   self-

    concepts  will be  denoted   as charac¬ter dimensions.

    The   temperament  dimensionswere defined   in terms of individualdifferences in associative learning inresponse  to novelty, danger or pun¬

    ishment, and reward. They have beendescribed previously in detail.18 Onetemperament   factor,  novelty seeking,is viewed   as a heritable   bias   in   theactivation or   initiation of behaviorssuch   as frequent exploratory activityin response to novelty, impulsive de¬cision  making,  extravagance   in   ap¬proach to cues of reward, and  quickloss of temper and  active avoidanceof frustration.   A   second   tempera¬ment factor, harm avoidance, is viewedas   a heritable  bias   in   the  inhibitionor cessation ofbehaviors, such  as pes¬simistic  worry   in anticipation of  fu¬ture problems,  passive avoidant  be¬haviors   such   as   fear of uncertaintyand  shyness of  strangers,  and  rapidfatigability.   The   third  temperamentfactor,   reward dependence,   is viewedas   a  heritable   bias   in   the   mainte¬nance   or   continuation  of ongoingbehaviors,   and   is manifest   as  senti¬mentality,   social  attachment,   and

    dependence  on approval of  others.Differences between individuals

    based on these dimensions are observ¬able in early childhood and are mod¬erately predictive of adolescent  andadult behavior.39 Accordingly,   theseaspects ofpersonality denote traits thatare usually considered temperamentfactors because they are heritable, mani¬fest early in  life,   and  apparently   in¬volve preconceptual or unconsciousbiases in

    learning.

    wnloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Virginia User on 07/27/2012

  • 8/20/2019 Psychobiological Model of Temperament and Character (1)

    4/16

    The  structure  of temperamentin   this   model   was   inferred   largelyfrom  genetic   studies  of personalityin   humans   and  neurobiologicalstudies of   the   functional   organiza¬tion   of   brain networks regulatingclassic   and   opérant   learning   re¬sponses   of rodents   to simple   ap¬

    petitive  or

      aversive   stimuli.18·40·41As   is typical   of   complex   systemsthat   are usually   hierarchical   anddecomposable   into   stable   sub¬systems that evolved sequentially,42ethologic   studies   also   suggestedthat   the   phylogeny   of   tempera¬ment began  with   a  behavioral   in¬hibition   (harm   avoidance)   systemin   all   animals,   next  added   an   acti¬vation   (novelty   seeking)   system   inmore   advanced   animals,   and   then

    added   subsystems   for  behavioralmaintenance   (reward  dependence)in reptiles   and   later phyla.43

    Normative   studies using   theTPQ,   a self-report  inventory  mea¬suring the   three dimensions of  tem¬perament   described   here,   con¬firmed   the   proposed   structure   oftemperament with the exception thatpersistence emerged  as  a distinct fourthdimension.22·23·44  Persistence,   origi¬nally thought   to be   a component  ofreward  dependence,  was  measuredin   terms ofperseverance despite frus¬tration and  fatigue; it  was uncorre-lated with other aspects of reward de¬

    pendence   (sentimentality,   socialattachment, and  dependence on ap¬proval). A large-scale twin study con¬firmed that each of  these  four   tem¬

    perament   factors   had heritabilitybetween 50% and 65% and  was ge¬netically homogeneous and indepen¬dent of the others.22   In contrast, per¬

    sonality   factors  derived  by   factoranalysis, such   as neuroticism and   ex¬traversion,   are composites  of   etio-logically  heterogeneous   items, withheritable variation  in  at least two  d i¬mensions besides the four TPQ   tem¬perament dimensions.  In other words,there are more than five heritable di¬mensions of personality.22

    Ethological  studies   also   sug¬gested   that   conceptual   or insight-based

     learning evolved after the pre-

    conceptual learning biases involvedin  temperament.43 Hence we  extendour theory  of personality   to  allowfor   the   development   of   concept-based   character   traits.   Character

    development   is   defined   here   interms of  insight learning or reorga¬nization   of   self-concepts.   Insight

    involves   the   conceptual   organiza¬tion   of   perception   and   is  definedas   the   apprehension   of   relation¬ships.   Insight  learning  involves   thedevelopment   of   a   new adaptiveresponse   as   a   result   of   a   sudden

    conceptual   reorganization   ofexperience.27·43   In   human  beings,insight   learning   includes   verballearning,   the   acquisition   of  learn¬ing   sets   or  how   to   learn,   and   ab¬stract conceptualization   that   influ¬ences   behavioral   goals   andexpectancies.

    Human   beings   process   orconvert   some sensory  inputs   (ie,percepts)   into  abstract symbols   (ie,concepts).   Consequently   in humanbeings,  stimulus-response   charac¬teristics depend   on   the  conceptualsignificance   and   salience   of   per¬ceived   stimuli.38   Hence  charactermay   be   described   in   terms  of   the

    response   biases  related   to  different

    concepts   of   the   self,   ie,  who   andwhat   we   are,   and   why   we arehere.   Our   unconscious automatic

    responses   to  initiate,   maintain,   or

    stop   behavior   are initially   deter¬mined   by   temperament   factors,but   these   can   be   modified   andconditioned   as   a   result  of  changesin   the   significance   and   salience   ofstimuli  that   are   determined by  ourconcept  of our identity.   From   thisperspective,   personality   develop¬ment   is   seen   as   an   iterative   epige-netic   process   in   which  heritable

    temperament  factors   initially  moti¬vate insight   learning   of   self-concepts,   which   in   turn modifythe significance   and   salience   ofperceived   stimuli   to   which   theperson responds.   In this way, bothtemperament   and   character   devel¬

    opment   influence   one   another  andmotivate   behavior.

    Three   aspects  of  the

      develop-

    ment of self-concepts (ie, charactero-logical   response   sets)   are  distin¬guished  according   to   the  extent towhich   a person identifies the  self   as

    (1)  an autonomous individual, (2)  an

    integral part of humanity or society,or (3) an integral  part of the unity ofall things (ie, the universe, which de¬

    notes everything turned into one in¬terdependent whole). Each aspect ofself-concept   corresponds   to   one   ofthree   character dimensions that wecall   (1)   self-directedness,   (2)   coop¬erativeness,   and   (3)   self-transcen¬dence,  respectively.

    Empirical   Dimensionsof Character

    After the genetic structure of the four

    temperament dimensions  was   con¬firmed, other studies were carried outto identify aspects of self-reported orobserver-rated personality that are notcorrelated with temperament   as mea¬sured  by   the   TPQ.   First,   in   factor-analytic research on personality  in  11-year-old children,   a factor defined byadjustment problems was   found  tobe largely uncorrelated with noveltyseeking,   harm  avoidance,   and   re¬ward dependence.24·39 Observationswere   based   on  behavioral   adjust¬ment ratings obtained in clinical in¬terviews of the children's teachers. The

    adjustment  problem   factor  was   la¬beled  "unpopularity" because   it washighly correlated with frequent peerconflicts   (re.75),   being  unpopular(r=.70),  and bullying or being  bul¬lied  (r=.64).   The  temperament  and

    popularity  factors   were   uncorre¬lated   with   intelligence   and   aca¬demic  achievement.

    In studies of self-reported  per¬sonality by adults, we  observed thatvarious measures of social  coopera¬tion  and   compassion were not  wellexplained  by  the  TPQ.   Specifically,the temperament  factors of the  TPQwere largely uncorrelated with somemeasures of social cooperation, suchas  the agreeability  scale of  the  Neu-roticism-Extraversion-Openness  per¬sonality inventory,46   the  aggressionscale  of   the   Multidimensional   Per-

    wnloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Virginia User on 07/27/2012

  • 8/20/2019 Psychobiological Model of Temperament and Character (1)

    5/16

    sonality Questionnaire (MPQ),18-47 andthe   hostility   scale   of the  Profile  ofMood  States.48

    Furthermore, measures of indi¬vidual self-acceptance and  esteem werenot well explained by the  TPQ. Thisincluded   the  alienation   scale  of the

    MPQ, in which individuals view them¬

    selves   as victims of other people andcircumstances.18·47 Italso includes therepression   factor of   the  MinnesotaMultiphasic  Psychological   Inven¬tory,   in which individuals have  dif¬

    ficulty admitting to faults about them¬selves.49 Acceptance  of  individualresponsibility, positive self-regard, andleadership have been found to be  cor¬related with the ability  to endorse un¬flattering  statements about self in otherwork.50

    Finally, absorption in the  MPQis  also  not  well correlated with anyTPQ factors or with any of the  three

    higher   order   dimensions   of  theMPQ.18·47  The  absorption   scale   hasbeen associated with differential re-sponsivity   to hypnosis,  meditation,biofeedback, and with increased   ca¬pacity   for  vivid   imagery.31  Absorp¬tion or self-forgetful concentration hasbeen described   as   a stage in  insightmeditation   that   leads   to   self-

    transcendence.52   It is  also describedas a correlate ofself-actualization, self-transcendence,   and   loving   fascina¬tion with   nature.53·54

    These  observations  suggestedthat  aspects  of personality  unmea¬sured  by   the   TPQ   include   dimen¬sions of character  related   to  accep¬tance of the individual self, acceptanceof other people,   and   acceptance  ofnature   in general.   Individuals withmature

    personalities (ie, effective ad¬

    aptation and self-satisfaction) are self-reliant, cooperative, and possibly self-transcendent.   In contrast, those with

    personality disorders have difficuttywith   self-acceptance,   are   intolerantand revengeful  toward  others,  andmay   feel  self-conscious   and  unful¬filled. This suggested the hypothesisthat  subtypes   of personality  disor¬der  can be  defined   in   terms of   tem¬

    perament   variables,18  whereas   the

    presence or absence of

    personality dis-

    order may be defined in terms of thecharacter   dimensions   of   self-

    directedness, cooperativeness, and self-transcendence.

    Description  of Character

    To develop   explicit   self-reportmeasures   of  self-directedness,   co-o pe ra t i vene ss ,   and   self-

    transcendence,   prior  descriptionsof   related   psychological   conceptswere  reviewed.  These provided   de¬scriptions   of   specific   componentfactors   for   each   of   the   three   pro¬posed  dimensions  of  character.

    Self-Directedness

    Our  interview  studies   indicate   that

    self-directedness  is  the major deter¬minant of   the   presence   or   absenceof personality   disorder.25  Accord¬ingly,  we   will consider   this  dimen¬sion  in the most detail because of itsclinical  importance.

    The basic concept ofself-directed¬ness refers to  self-determination and

    "willpower,"  or  the ability of  an in¬dividual to control, regulate, and adaptbehavior   to   fit   the   situation   in   ac¬cord with individually chosen goalsand values.55 Popular concepts about"willpower"  can be confusing,  how¬ever, because willpower or the "will"is not  an entity,  but   a metaphoricalabstract concept   to describe  the   ex¬tent to which   a person identifies the

    imaginai   self   as   an integrated,   pur¬poseful whole individual, rather thana disorganized   set   of   reactive   im¬pulses. A more neutral and   informa¬tive term than willpower may  be  torefer   to   the  intentional   force of in¬dividuals to affirm or commit  to par¬ticular goals or purposes. Accordingto  humanists,  human beings  differfrom other animais in terms of their

    ability to make choices among alter¬native   responses,   to "change   theirmind," and  to tell lies; therefore, hu¬man beings  can  be  considered   to beresponsible   for what they  do.56

    On  the positive side, individu¬als who are mature, effective, and well-

    organized leaders are described  as hav-

    ing  good  self-esteem,   able   to admitfaults and  accept themselves   as theyare,  feel that their lives  have  mean¬

    ing  and  purpose, delay gratificationto achieve their goals, and show ini¬tiative in overcoming challenges. Onthe  negative side, "borderline" indi¬viduals have low  self-esteem, blame

    others   for their  problems,   feel   un¬certain of their identity or purpose,and are often reactive, dependent, andresourceless. Such low self-esteem and

    feelings of inferiority have been sug¬gested to  be   immature or "childish"characteristics.37  However, many chil¬dren show positive self-esteem   at anearly age, particularly when their par¬ents show consistent affection and  en¬

    courage autonomy.58,59More specifically,   Rotter60   de¬

    scribed  the   notion of  locus  of con¬trol, differentiating those with an in¬ternal locus of control (who believetheir  success   is controlled  by theirown efforts) from those with an   ex¬ternal locus of control (who believetheir success is controlled by factorsother than themselves). Studies of thismeasure showed that those  with in¬ternal  locus of control  are more   re¬

    sponsible  and   resourceful problem-solvers,   whereas   others   are  morealienated   and  apathetic,   tending   toblame other people  and chance   cir¬cumstances   for   problems.61   Somemeasures of locus of control are con¬founded  with other aspects of   tem¬

    perament   and  mood   (eg,   depres¬sion,   high   harm   avoidance,   anddependence   on approval  and   per¬suasion), but   a factor of responsibil¬ity vs blaming appears to be distinctfrom other   aspects of temperamentand related to  the more

    general con¬

    cept of   self-directedness.Frankl62   has   emphasized   the

    importance   of   purpose fulness   andmeaningful goal-direction   as a moti¬vating   force   in  mature people.   Hesuggested   that man's   main concernis to fulfill meaning, rather than togratifyimpulses   and   avoid   conflicts.   Suchpurposefulness varies widely amongindividuals.63 Rogers64 and Covey65 haveemphasized   initiative  and  resource¬ful

      problem   solving  in   effective

    wnloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Virginia User on 07/27/2012

  • 8/20/2019 Psychobiological Model of Temperament and Character (1)

    6/16

    schoolchildren  and business   lead¬

    ers,  respectively,   as   an importantaspect of mature character. Bandura66and  Bandura   and   Cervone67   de¬scribed   self-efficacy,  which   is   re¬lated   to   resourcefulness   and   initia¬tive   in goal-directed   behavior.According   to  some Yoga  practition¬ers, after someone has cultivated cleargoals and values for  a long time, whatwas initially an effort becomes   "sec¬ond nature."68   In other words, such

    integrated individuals expect their au¬tomatic responses   to   be   congruentwith their goals and values, and theytrust   themselves   to   act spontane¬ously  without   any feeling   of sup¬pressed conflict.

    Self-esteem and the ability to  ac¬cept one's limitations unapologeticallywithout fantasies ofunlimited abilityand ageless youth are crucial aspectsof  the   development of mature   self-directed behavior.53·57·58·64 individualswith poor adjustment and feelings ofinferiority or inadequacy are often re¬active and deny, repress, or ignore theirfaults, wishing to be best at everythingalways, whereas well-adjustedchildrenare  able to recognize and admit un¬

    flattering truths about   themselves.50Such positive self-esteem and abilityto accept individual limitations has beenfound to be  strongly correlated withresponsibility and   resourcefulness.50

    In summary, self-directedness can

    be formulated  as  a developmental pro¬cess with several stages oraspects. Theseinclude (1) acceptance of responsibil¬ity  for  one's own  choices   instead ofblaming  other   people   and  circum¬stances, (2) identification of individu¬ally valued goals and purposes vs lackof goal direction, (3) development ofskills and confidence in solving prob¬lems  (resourcefulness vs apathy), (4)self-acceptance  vs self-striving, and fi¬nally (5) congruent second nature vspersonal  distrust.  Sample  questionsfrom the  Temperament and  Charac¬ter Inventory (TCI) are listed inTable  1alongwith theirendorsement frequencyin a community sample described later.The   assessment ofself-directedness iscrucial for diagnosis because low self-directedness  is   the   common   character¬istic of all categories of personality  dis¬orders,   as shown in our companion ar¬ticle.25 Regardless ofother personalitytraits or circumstances, personality dis¬order is likely  to   be  present   if self-directedness  is low,  and  vice versa.

    Cooperativeness

    The  second  higher  order   characterfactor of cooperativeness was formu¬lated   to account   for individual   dif¬ferences  in identification with and  ac¬

    ceptance of other people. This factor

    was designed to measure different as¬pects of character related   to   agree-

    ability vs self-centered aggression andhostility.   In our companion   article,we   found  that low   cooperativenessscores contribute substantially to thelikelihood of  concomitant personal¬ity  disorder.23  Particularly   in   indi¬

    viduals who are high or only mod¬erately low   in  self-directedness,   theprobability of  a diagnosis of person¬ality disorder was  increased  by lowcooperativeness. All categories of per¬sonality disorder are associated with lowcooperativeness.

    Cooperative individuals are  de¬scribed   as socially  tolerant,   em¬pathie, helpful,  and   compassionate,whereas uncooperative individuals aredescribed   as socially intolerant,  dis¬interested   in  other people,  unhelp¬ful, and revengeful. Rogers64 has  de¬scribed facilitative people who showunconditional acceptance  of  others,empathy  with   others'   feelings,   andwillingness  to help others achieve theirgoalswithout selfish domination. Suchsocial   acceptance,  helpfulness,   andconcern  for   the   rights of  others   areoften correlated with one another andwith  positive  self-esteem.69  Empa¬thy, defined   as   a feeling of unity oridentification with  other  people,   issaid to allow improved communica¬tion   and compassion   for  others.70

    wnloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Virginia User on 07/27/2012

  • 8/20/2019 Psychobiological Model of Temperament and Character (1)

    7/16

    Helpful  generativity  and compas¬sion are frequently noted   as signs ofmaturity in  developmental psychol¬ogy.71  Such compassion involves thewillingness to forgive and be kind toothers  regardless  of their  behavior,rather than to seek revenge or to en¬

     joy their  embarrassment   or   suffer¬

    ing;   it  involves feelings of  brotherlylove and  the  absence of hostility.53·72

    Effective business leadership of¬ten involves helpfulness,   ie,  skill infinding  mutually   satisfying   ("win-win")  solutions   to problems,   ratherthan  looking out  only for personalgain.63 Religious traditions from Bud¬dhism to Judeo-Christianity also em¬phasize   the   concept   of   "pure-hearted" acceptance of principles ornatural  laws that  cannot  be  broken

    without   inevitable   bad   conse¬

    quences   for  the   individual,   society,and  nature.65·73  Belief in  such   natu¬ral principles,   as opposed   to imme¬diate self-advantage  or   social   con¬ventions, represents an advanced levelof moral or character  development,as   described  by   Kohlberg74·75   andBaruk.76

    In summary, cooperativeness canbe formulated  as  a developmental  pro¬cess  with   several

      aspects  or stages.

    These include   (1)  social  acceptancevs  intolerance, (2) empathy vs socialdisinterest,   (3)  helpfulness   vs  un-

    helpfulness,   (4)   compassion   vs   re-vengefulness,   and   (5)   pure-heartedprinciples vs self-advantage. Samplequestions about  cooperativeness arelisted  in Table 2. Uncooperative  in¬dividuals tend to   see  the world and

    others   as hostile  and  alien   to   them.In contrast,   cooperative individuals

    feel they are synergistic componentsof   a mutually supportive   and  help¬ful community that   is motivated bycompassion and   reciprocal   respect.

    Self-Transcendence

    Most people  meditate or pray daily,which  is  more frequent  than sexualintercourse according to populationsurveys.77  Frequent   meditation   orprayer   is  often  associated   with   en¬

    hanced life satisfaction and personaleffectiveness,  particularly  in  olderadults.78   Nevertheless,   self-transcendence and character traits as¬

    sociated with   spirituality  have  usu¬ally   been  neglected   in   systematicresearch   and  omitted from person¬ality   inventories  that purport  to   becomprehensive,   including   the   five-factor model. However, much infor¬mation about the  observable behav¬ior of self-transcendent  individualshas been  written  based on   the   livesof mystics   and   saints79·80   and   self-actualizing   individuals.53·54   Also

    transpersonal  psychology   has   re¬cently provided  descriptions  of thesubjective   experiences  and  changesin behavior of people who attain thestate   of "nirvana"  or   self-transcen¬dence   as   a result of insight  medita¬tion techniques.81·82   Self-transcen¬dence refers generally to identificationwith everything conceived   as   essen¬tial and  consequential parts of a uni¬fied whole.  This involves   a   state  of

    "unitive consciousness"  in which  ev¬

    erything is part ofone totality.  In uni¬tive consciousness,   there   is no indi¬

    vidual   self   because   there   is   no

    meaningful  distinction between selfand other—the person is simply awareof being an integral  part of  the evo¬lution of the cosmos. This unitive per¬spective may be described as accep¬tance, identification, or spiritual unionwith nature  and   its   source.79

    We found that self-transcendencewas lower   in psychiatric   inpatientsthan adults   in   the  general  commu¬nity, but did not   differentiate  mostpatients  with   personality   disordersfrom   those   without   such   disor¬ders.25   In  the  clinical  setting of  ourinterview   studies   with  psychiatricpatients,  self-transcendence   was

    significantly  low   only   in   patientswith  many  symptoms   of  schizoid

    personality   disorder.23   In   contrast,self-directedness   and  cooperative-

    wnloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Virginia User on 07/27/2012

  • 8/20/2019 Psychobiological Model of Temperament and Character (1)

    8/16

    ness   were   low   in   all personalitydisorders.  Self-transcendence   can

    be  particuiarly  useful   in  differenti¬ating schizoid from schizotypal   pa¬tients   because   the   latter   tend   toendorse questions   about   extrasen¬sory   perception   and  other   aspectsof self-transcendence.

    The  stable self-forgetfulness ofself-transcendent people has been de¬scribed   as   the   same   as experiencedtransiently by people when they aretotally  absorbed,   intensely   concen¬trated, and  fascinated by one thing.54In   such  one-pointed   concentrationpeople may forget where they are andlose all sense of the  passage of time.

    Such absorption often   leads to"transpersonal" identificationwith thingsoutside the individual self. The per¬sonmay identify (or feel a sense ofspiri¬tual union)   with anything or every¬thing. They may experience the feel¬ing that they are part oforbeing guidedby  a wonderful intelligence, which ispossibly the divine source of all phe¬nomena. Ultimately, there maybe lossofall distinctions between selfand other

    by identifying with the concept of animmanent  God   as one-in-all.

    Such transpersonal identificationleads   to spiritual   acceptance, or the

    apprehension of relationships that can¬not be explained by analytical reason¬ing or demonstrated by objective  ob¬servation to others. Spirituality has beendefined  as our inner craving to be im-

    mortal,83 which   leads  us   to identifywith nature as a whole or with its source.Such arguably metaphorical conceptsas extrasensory perception or reincar¬nation of souls can be understood  as

    attempts  to  describe the   experienceof transpersonal identification usingwords,   which   unavoidably   denote

    things and events. Use of language todescribe self-transcendent experienceis   difficult   at   best   because   self-transcendentcognition is intuitive ratherthan analytical and  deductive.83

    Considered   as a developmentalprocess, self-transcendence has mul¬

    tiple  aspects or stages.  These   stagescan be simplified into some basic  ex¬periences and behaviors that have beendescribed in abroad spectrum ofpeopleand cultures, not just rare mystics: (1)

    self-forgetful vs self-conscious expe¬rience, (2) transpersonal identification(ie, identification with nature) vs self-differentiation, and   (3)   spiritual   ac¬ceptance vs rational materialism. Some

    examples ofquestions about these fac¬tors are listed in Table 3. In our clini¬cal studies only the symptoms ofschiz¬oid personality disorder were signifi¬cantly   correlated   with   low   self-transcendence, so it is  not  a commoncharacteristic of traditional conceptsofpersonality disorder.25 Nevertheless,current DSM-U1-R definitions of per¬

    sonality disorder may be too narrowbecause spiritualityand other phenom¬ena measured by this dimension are

    important for the adjustment and per¬sonal satisfaction ofmany people, par¬ticularly those over 35 years of age.84Self-transcendence merits further sys¬tematic study   in samples from bothclinical and general populations to clarifyits clinical s ignificance.

    Empirical Testing of thePersonality Model

    A sample of 300 adults,   150  womenand   150   men,  completed   the   TCI,which is a 226-item, true-false ques¬tionnaire   measuring   seven   dimen¬sions of personality.  These include   a107-item  version of  the  TPQ,   mea¬suring the  temperament dimensionsof novelty seeking, harm avoidance,and reward dependence and  persis¬tence,  ft also includes  a  119-item char¬acter inventory  measuring   self-directedness, cooperativeness, and self-transcendence.  These  119 items were

    selected from 195 items obtained bygenerating   15  questions for  each ofthe   13 rationally defined   characterfactors.  This included measures of thefive aspects of self-directedness, fiveaspects of cooperativeness, and  fiveaspects   of  self-transcendence   de¬scribed  in   the  prior  section.   Thesemeasures were pretested in  a sampleof 212 college   students,  discardingquestions   that   had  extreme   re¬sponse   frequencies   (generally   out¬side   20%   to   80%   endorsement)  or

    wnloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Virginia User on 07/27/2012

  • 8/20/2019 Psychobiological Model of Temperament and Character (1)

    9/16

    were weakly correlated with the otherretained  items on each factor. No  se¬

    lection was made based on  intercor-relations between factors. This yielded13  scales that are internally   consis¬tent  and   face-valid  measures of theconstructs  to  be   tested  here.

    Subjects were solicited for  par¬ticipation   in   the  study   as they   en¬tered   a shopping mall  whose   cus¬tomers  were thought   to   be fairlyrepresentative  of the general   popu¬lation of St Louis, Mo. Subjects aged

    18  years or older were recruited   se¬

    quentially,   and  were excluded onlyif they were in an oversampled gender-age   group.   The   recruiters   and   ad¬ministrators of the   test were blind tothe personality model  measured bythe TCI.  The mean (±SD) age of thesample was 34.1 ± 12.9 years (range,18 to  91 years).  The women   (mean[±SD],   35.5±13.7 years;  range,   18to 91 years) were slightly older thanthe  men (mean   [±SD],   32.7±11.9

    years; range,   18   to   84 years).   The

    sample   consisted  of  114 white   (62men and  52 women)  and   186 non-white (88  men and  98 women) sub¬

     jects.To   test for representativeness of

    the   sample,   the   results  of   the   TPQwere compared with  those obtainedin a national area probability sampleof 1019 noninstitutionalized adults.23

    Statistical   analyses   were   car¬ried out using version 6.03 of the SASstatistical software (SAS Institute, Cary,NC).85

    wnloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Virginia User on 07/27/2012

  • 8/20/2019 Psychobiological Model of Temperament and Character (1)

    10/16

    * Correlations above  .40   are  indicated in  bold.   S1  indicates   responsibility  vs blaming; S2,   purposefulness   vs   lack of goal direction;  S3,  resourcefulness; S4,self-acceptance  vs  self-striving; S5,  congruent second nature;  C1,  social acceptance   vs social  intolerance;  C2,  empathy   vs social  disinterest;  C3,  helpfulnessvs unhelpfulness;  C4,   compassion   vs revengefulness; C5,  pure-hearted principles   vs self advantage;  ST1,  self-forgetful  vs  self-conscious   experience;  ST2,

    transpersonal  Identification   vs self-differentiation; and ST3,  spiritual acceptance  vs rational materialism.

    RESULTS

    Variability  of Personality   Traits

    The   means,  SDs,   and   internal  con¬

    sistency (Cronbach a) for each scaleand   subscale   are   shown   in

    Tables  4   and   5   for  the   tempera¬ment   and character   scales,   respec¬

    tively.  The results   for  the   tempera¬ment   scales  were  similar   to   thoseobtained in  a national area probabil¬ity   sample,23   suggesting   that   thesample was representative of the  gen¬eral adult population.   All  the   scalesshowed substantial variability amongindividuals.   The   internal   consis¬

    tency of the composite scales was high,ranging from .76 to  .87 for the   tem¬perament   scales   and   .84  to   .89  forthe  character  scales.

    Structure   of Character   Scales

    The   correlations   among   the   13

    character   subscales   are   summa¬rized   in   Table   6. Principal   com¬ponent   analysis   identified   threefactors   with   eigenvalues   greaterthan   1.0.   These   accounted   for

    35%,   16%,   and  8%  of the  variance(59%   cumulatively).   The  standard¬ized   factor

      loadings  following Pro-

    max   rotation   (that   is,   allowing   fornonindependence   of   factors)   areshown   in   Table   7.   The   highestloadings  of all   the   cooperativenessscales   and   self-acceptance   are   onfactor   1.   In other  words,   the   abil¬ity  to accept  limitations about  one¬self is  associated with   the  ability   totolerate and   accept   limitations   in

    other  people   as   well.   The   highestloadings of all   the   self-directednesssubscales   except  self-acceptanceare   on   factor   2.   The  highest   load¬ings   of all   the  self-transcendencescales   are   on   factor   3.   The   vari¬ances explained   by   each   factor   af¬ter   rotation  were 30%,   27%,   andi5%,   respectively.   Interfactor   cor¬relations   were   .52   for   factors   fand   2,   —.16   for   factors   2   and   3,and   .06   for   factors   1   and   3.   The

    derived   three-factor   solution   forthe   f3   character   scales   corre¬

    sponds   closely  with   the   rationallydefined   dimensions   of self-

    directedness,   cooperativeness,   andself-transcendence.

    Structure   of Temperamentand Character

    The correlations   among   the   four

    dimensions   of  temperament

      and

    three   dimensions   of  character   aresummarized   in  Table   8.   The  onlycorrelations   above   .40   relate  harmavoidance   with  self-directedness

    ( —

    .47)   and   cooperativeness   withreward dependence   (.54)   and   self-directedness   (.57).

    Principal  component   analysisidentified seven   factors with  eigen¬values   greater   than   1.   fn   the   vari-max   solution   the   variances   ex¬

    plained   by   each   factor   were14.2%, 12.0%,   10.1%,   9.0%,7.6%,   6.0%,  and   5.7%,   accountingfor   65.0%   of   the   total   variance.

    The   standardized factor   loadingsfollowing   Promax   rotation   areshown   in   Table   9.   Ignoring   cor¬relations   with   other   factors,   thevariances   explained   by   each   ro¬tated   factor   ranged   from   4.4%   to2.0%,  which   is   17.7%   to   8.0%   ofthe  total variance   in   the  25   factors.The  correlations   among   the   seven

    empirically derived   factors   corre¬sponded closely to those summarizedin Table 8  for the rationally definedfactors.

    The   persistence   factor   appearsto be   a fairly distinct seventh dimen¬sion  of personality,  but   it   has   sig¬nificant relations to other aspects ofboth

     temperament and  character.   It

    wnloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Virginia User on 07/27/2012

  • 8/20/2019 Psychobiological Model of Temperament and Character (1)

    11/16

    *The highest loadings are  indicated in  bold.   S1  indicates responsibility  vs  blaming; S2,  purposefulnessvs  lack of goal direction; S3,  resourcefulness; S4,   self-acceptance   vs self-striving;  S5,   congruent secondnature; C1,  social acceptance vs  social  intolerance; C2,  empathy   vs social disinterest; C3,  helpfulness vsunhelpfulness;  C4,  compassion  vs  revengefulness; C5,  pure-hearted principles  vs  self advantage; ST1,self-forgetful  vs  self-conscious experience; ST2,  transpersonal identification  vs  self-differentiation; andST3,  spiritual acceptance  vs  rational materialism.

    *Correlations above .40  are   indicated in  bold.   NS indicates novelty seeking;  HA,  harm avoidance; P,persistence; RD,   reward dependence;  SD,   self-directedness; C ,  cooperativeness; and  ST,self-transcendence.

    has positive correlations greater than.20 with three self-directedness com¬

    ponents:   resourcefulness (.36),  con¬gruent second nature (.28), and pur¬posefulness  (.24). However,   it is not

    strongly  correlated   with  other   as¬

    pects of self-directedness such   as re-

    sponsibility   (.14)   and   self-acceptance   (.08).   Persistence   hasnegative correlations less than  —.20with three temperament  factors:   fa-

    tigability   ( —

    .29),   impulsiveness( —

    .21),   and  disorderliness   ( —

    .21).In   this   sample,   it   was  not signifi¬cantly   correlated   with   reward-dependence  measures  such   as   sen¬timentality  (.09),   attachment   (.03),or

    dependence   ( — .07).

      We   tenta-

    tively consider persistence to be   a tem¬perament factor because of its promi¬nent development in early childhood.

    Effects of  DemographicVariables on Character

    The effects of gender, ethnicity, andage were examined on the  characterscales.  Women had higher scores ontotal cooperativeness   (81%   vs   72%as proportion of total items; P

  • 8/20/2019 Psychobiological Model of Temperament and Character (1)

    12/16

    *Highest loadings   are given   in  bold; postulated factors,   in italics.   NS1   indicates  exploratory excitability; NS2,  impulsiveness; NS3,  extravagance; NS4,disorderliness;  HA1,  worry/pessimism; HA2,   fear of uncertainty;  HA3,  shyness  with  strangers;  HA4,  fatigability and  asthenia;  RD1,  sentimentality   vsinsensitivity; RD2,  persistence;  RD3,   attachment  vs  detachment;  RD4,   dependence   vs independence;  S1,  responsibility   vs blaming; S2,  purposefulness;  S3 ,resourcefulness;  S4,  self-acceptance   vs self-striving; S5,   congruent second  nature;  C1,   social acceptance  vs social  intolerance;  C2,   empathy;  C3,   helpfulness;04,   compassion   vs revengefulness;  C5,  pure-hearted principles;  ST1,  self-forgetful  vs   self-conscious   experience;  ST2,   transpersonal identification   vsself-differentiation; and  ST3,  spiritual acceptance   vs  rational materalism.

    development   of increasingly   inclu¬sive  concepts of the   self:  identifica¬tion   as   an  autonomous   individual

    (self-directedness),   as   an integralpart  of  human   society   (coopera¬tiveness),  and   as  an integral  part ofthe   universe   (self-transcendence).This   supports   the   hypothesis   thatpersonality   is   a complex   hierarchicsystem   that   can   be  naturally   decom¬posed   into   distinct  psychobiologicaldimensions   of   temperament   andcharacter.

    Alternative Models

    We   do  not   assume   that   the   psy¬chobiological   model   presentedherein   provides   the   only   way   toderive  a description and understand¬ing  of character   traits.   In   fact,   psy-

    chodynamic  theories

      suggest  that

    character   traits   arise   as  stable   resi¬

    dues   of  normal   defense   mecha¬

    nisms,   such   as anticipation,   altru¬ism,  and  sublimation.  Anticipation,which  enables  people   to   work  forlong-term   goals,   can   lead   to   someself-directed behaviors   like   pur-

    posefulness.   Likewise   altruism   canlead   to   cooperative   traits,   such   as

    helpfulness   and   compassion.   De¬spite   such   parallels   in   content,psychodynamic   concepts   of   char¬acter  are categorical  constructs  thatemphasize   the   uniqueness   of eachindividual.   In contrast,  we empha¬size   the   consistent   quantitativestructure   of  the   differences   amongindividuals.   This   dimensionalstructure   facilitates   the   testing   ofquantitative,   falsifiable   hypothesesrelating   psychological   variation   toits

    biological  and   social   causes.

    Development

    The   finding of three distinct charac¬ter dimensions has strong implicationsfor models of  longitudinal develop¬ment.   The   findings   of   Erikson71·86and others   are often   interpreted   tomean that   there   is   a   fixed   stepwisesequence   in which   development  ofone   factor  necessarily   precedes   thedevelopment  of the   next.  Bowlby87has  suggested   an alternative   epige-netic  model   in  which   personalitydevelopment   of   each   individualcan proceed  along any  one of   a   setof  potential   paths,   depending   oninitial   temperament   and   initial   ex¬

    perience.   According   to   thismultiple-path   epigenetic   model,each   subsequent   step   in develop¬ment   is   a motivated effort   to adaptto   current   circumstances,   given

    wnloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Virginia User on 07/27/2012

  • 8/20/2019 Psychobiological Model of Temperament and Character (1)

    13/16

    85

    g   75

    65

    55

    45

    Resourceful

    Congruent

    Responsible

    Purposeful

    Self-accepting

    15   20 —

    25 —

    30 —

    35 — -

    40 — "

    45 — "

    50 — -

    55 —I

    60

    Age, y

    SD1

     SD  2

     

    SD  3

     SD  4•SD 5

    Figure   1.  Relationship of  age   to  self-directedness (SD)  subscale   scores  in  quintiles of 300 individualsfrom   the general community.

    Age,  y

    C1

    C2

    C3

    C4

    C5

    Figure   2.  Relationship of age  to  cooperativeness   (C)  subscale   scores in  quintiles of 300 individualsfrom   the general  community.

    current

    personality. However, if there

    really  were   a   fixed  developmentalsequence for character traits, then ma¬

    turity  could   be   adequately   de¬scribed on  a single  scale. The  obser¬vation of three character dimensions

    suggests   that   there   are multiple   as¬pects of character development, eachhaving  unique   antecedents.   Never¬theless,   the joint  staging and   inter¬action of these multiple dimensionshas   received  little  study  because  of

    the absence of comprehensive quan-

    titative measures of the

     multiple  fac¬

    tors  of temperament  and  character.Some sequences   in character   devel¬

    opment may be more frequent or op¬timal   than   others,  and  the   correla¬tions   among   the   character   and

    temperament  dimensions   suggestsome reciprocal interaction. Our find¬ings   about   longitudinal   develop¬ment should  be  considered   illustra¬tive   and   tentative  because   this   is   across-sectional study of people ofvary¬

    ing age.  In

     this study we cannot

     dis-

    tinguish developmental effects fromcohort effects. Nevertheless, our find¬ings   suggest   the   importance  of ageand/or cohort effects   in   the  unfold¬

    ing  of character and   encourage   fu¬ture longitudinal  research.

    DIAGNOSIS

    These   character  scales  may help   toclarify current concepts ofwhat  a per¬sonality disorder is  in  contrast to moreoptimal  adaptation.   That   is,   indi¬viduals with extreme variants of tem¬

    perament maybe well-adapted, depend¬ing  on   their   character and  circum¬stances. However, anyone who is lowin  self-directedness and  cooperative¬ness is likely to have a personality dis¬order, and vice versa. The importanceof self-transcendence  in character de¬

    velopment is arguable in early life, butit becomes a major concern as we facedeath and misfortune.  The  availabil¬

    ity of this  set of personality measuresshould facilitate evaluation and  clas¬sification of personality and its disor¬ders,  as well  as studies of inheritance,information processing, and develop¬ment (cognitive, personal, social, moral,and  spiritual).

    The three  dimensions of  char¬acter are distinct from previously de¬scribed   measures  of temperament.Overall, our results support   a seven-dimensional model ofpersonality; fourtemperament factors and  three char¬acter  factors.  The   temperament   fac¬tors appear to  be  more directly tiedto neurobiological   and  genetic   de¬terminants of behavior,   in  contrastto the epigenetic development of self-

    concepts in character. Furthermore,

    the   temperament  dimensions   ap¬pear to be more closely related to  sus¬ceptibility to different  neurotic syn¬dromes,   such   as anxiety   andsomatoform disorders, rather than tothe   presence  or  absence  of person¬ality  disorders   or psychoses.40  Eat¬ing disorders and substance abuse dis¬orders appear   to involve differencesin both temperament and character

    development, but this   requires   fur¬

    ther systematic study.19

    wnloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Virginia User on 07/27/2012

  • 8/20/2019 Psychobiological Model of Temperament and Character (1)

    14/16

    75

    70-

    65

    60

    55·

    50

    45   — -20

     — ~25 30

     — -35

     — -

    40 — ~

    45 — -

    50~ "

    55

    ge,  y

     —

    60

    ST  1

     

    ST  2

    ' ST 3

    Figure 3. Relationship of age   to   self-transcendence  (ST)   subscale   scores  in  quintiles of 300individuals   from   the general community.

    INHERITANCE

    It   is likely   that   genetic   factors   areas  important   in  character   develop¬ment   as they   are   for   temperament.In   fact,   the heritability of  charactermay explain  why  some  individualsmaintain inflexible maladaptive   be¬havior   patterns   whereas   otherswith similar   temperaments  do not.

    Conceptual  learning,   such   as   self-aware   imitation,   is   an evolutionarydevelopment  of  mammals.43   Indi¬vidual   differences   in  human   twinsfor   the   MPQ   primary   factors   ofalienation,  aggression,   and   absorp¬tion   are   influenced  by   genetic   fac¬tors   as  much   as  other   aspects  of

    personality.88 However,   if  culturalperspectives89·90  and social   learn¬ing39·64·66·91   are   as important   in   theepigénesis  of  self-concepts   as   hasbeen

     suggested,  then   environmen¬

    tal   effects  associated  with   particu¬lar   families   and  cultures  should   bemore important   in   character  devel¬opment   than   has   been   observedfor   temperament.   Comparisons   ofthe   inheritance   of  temperamentand   character   should  be  useful   for

    testing   sociocultural   learning   hy¬potheses.   Likewise,   the   effect   ofdifferent   forms   of  psychotherapyon   character  development   shouldbe   a

      controlled  way  of

     assessing

    the   influence   of   environmental

    change   on personality.

    MEMORY  SYSTEMS

    The most fundamental distinction be¬tween   character and   temperamenthere appears to be that character de¬

    velopment   is   a concept-based   pro¬cess  whereas temperament   involvesdifferences between individuals in  per¬

    ceptual  processes  and  habit   forma¬tion.   This   corresponds   to   the   dis¬tinction   of  conceptual   memory(regulated   by   the   cortico-limbo-diencephalic  memory  system)   andperceptual memory (regulated by thecortico-striatal  memory system).

    TREATMENT

    Different pharmacological interven¬tions have been

    proposed in the modi¬

    fication of temperament.40·92  For   ex¬ample, lithium therapy reduces   fre¬quency   of   temper   outbursts   andincreases   reflectiveness.93  However,lithium therapy does not change self-concepts or increase self-directedness,and compliance outside ofauthoritar¬ian controlled settings is poor.93 In con¬trast, different psychological treatmentsmay be relevant for the developmentof different aspects of character. For

    example, particularcognitive-behavioral

    techniques may facilitate learning self-directed behavior.53 Some other meth¬ods of psychological  treatment, suchas psychoanalysis, transactional analy¬sis, and reality  therapy,   are  also  di¬rected primarily at development ofself-directed behavior.81   fn contrast, other

    experiential techniques are designedto facilitate acceptance of others or de¬velopment of  cooperative behavior;these include Rogerian counseling,64logotherapy,62 and interpersonal psy-chosynthesis.94 Finally, attainment ofself-transcendence is  a goal ofJungiananalysis84 and insight meditation   aspracticed   in Mahayana   Buddhism,Vedanta Hinduism, Taoism, and mys¬tical forms of other religions.32 Muchresearch is needed to explore the  in¬teractions of specific drugs and psy¬chosocial interventions in treatmentof personality and   its  disorders.

    Finally,  assuming   that  charac¬ter and temperament involve concept-based   and   percept-driven  memory,stable personality change probably   re¬quires that conceptual insights modifyhabits by disciplined   practice,   per¬haps  facilitated by combined  phar-macotherapy.   In  other  words,   per¬sonality change has both rational andemotional components.95 Consciousintention   is transient,  effortful,   andinefficient, whereas perceptual  con¬ditioning is more long-lasting,   auto¬matic,   and   efficient.34·96·97 A  combi¬nation   of  cognitive-behavioraltreatments   and,   perhaps   only   ini¬tially,   medications   to  alter indi¬vidual   differences   in   temperamentshould be  most effective.

    Accepted for publication November

     4,1992.This study  was supported  in part

    by grant MH31302 from   the  NationalInstitute of Mental  Health,  and grantsAA07982  and AA08028 from   the  Na¬tional  Institute  on  Alcoholism  and  Al¬

    cohol Abuse, National Institutes of Health,Bethesda, Md, and a grant from  the JohnD.  and Catherine  T.  MacArthur Foun¬dation   Mental   Health   Research   Net¬work on   the Psychoblology of Depres¬

    sion,  Chicago,  III.

    wnloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Virginia User on 07/27/2012

  • 8/20/2019 Psychobiological Model of Temperament and Character (1)

    15/16

    Helpful  comments   were   receivedfrom many colleagues, particularly Sam¬uel  Guze, MD, John Nemiah, MD, andNorman   Sartorius,   MD,   and  our   de¬

    partmental  Personality  Study  Group(Kenneth Freedland, MD, Andrew Heath,MD,  Kim McCaüum,  MD, Joan Luby,MD, John Rohrbaugh,  MD,   and  Rich¬ard  Wetzel,  PhD).  We also appreciatethe   comments of Mary   Blehar,   PhD,Michael  Bohman,  MD,   Lindon   Eaves,PhD,  Peter Joyce,   MD,  Alex Kaplan,MD, Roger Mulder, MD, Gordon Parker,MD,   and  Soren Sigvardsson,   PhD.

    Reprint requests to Department ofPsychiatry, Washington University SchoolofMedicine, 4940 Children's Pi,  St Louis,MO 63110   (Dr  Cloninger).

    REFERENCES

    1.   American   Psychiatric  Association,   Committeeon Nomenclature and  Statistics. Diagnostic andStatistical  Manual  of  Mental  Disorders,   Re-vised Third Edition. Washington,  DC:  AmericanPsychiatric  Association;   1987.

    2.   Pfohl B,  Blum N,  Zimmerman M,  Stangl  D.  Struc-tured Interview for DSM-III-R Personality Dis-orders.  Iowa City,   Iowa: University of Iowa;  1989.

    3.   Loranger AW, Lenzenweger MF, Gartner AF, Sus-man   VL,   Herzeg   J,  Zammit   GK,  Gartner   JD,Abrams  RC,  Young  RC.  Trait-state artifacts andthe diagnosis of  personality disorders. Arch Gen

    Psychiatry.  1991;48:720-728.4.   Livesley WJ. A systematic approach to  the de-lineation of  personality   disorders. Am   J  Psy-chiatry.   1987;144:772-777.

    5.  Schroeder   ML,  Wormworth   JA,   Livesley   WJ.Dimensions of  personality  disorder   and   theirrelationships  to  the  big  five dimensions of  per-sonality.  Psychol Assess.   1992;4:47-53.

    6.   Clark   LA.   Toward   a  consensual   set   of   symp-tom clusters for assessment of personality dis-order. Adv Pers Assess.   1990;9:243-266.

    7.   Eysenck   HJ,  Eysenck   SB.   Manual of  the  EPQ(Eysenck  Personality  Questionnaire).   San   Di-ego,   Calif:   Educational   and   Industrial  TestingService;   1976.

    8.   Costa   PT  Jr,   McCrae   RR.   Four  ways five   fac-tors   are   basic. Pers   Individual  Diff.   1992;13:653-665.

    9.   Tellegen A.  Structures of mood and personalityand their relevance to assessing anxiety, with anemphasis on self-report.  In:  Tuma   AH, Maser J,eds. Anxiety and the Anxiety Disorders. Hillsdale,NJ:   Lawrence Erlbaum  Assoc;   1985:681-706.

    10.   Costa   PT Jr ,   McCrae   RR.   Normal   personalityassessment   in   clinical   practice:   the   NEO   per-sonality   inventory.  Psychol Assess.   1992;4:5\x=req-\13.

    11.   Trull T.  DSM-III-R personality disorders and thefive factor model  of  personality. J Abnorm Psy-chol.   1992;101:553-560.

    12.   Ben-Porath YS, Waller  NG.   Normal personality

    inventories   in   clinical  assessment:  general   re-quirements  and  the  potential for using the  NEOpersonality inventory. Psychol  Assess.   1992;4:14-19.

    13.   John   OP.   The  search  for  basic   dimensions  of

    personality:   review  and  critique.   Adv  PsycholAssess.   1990;7:1-37.

    14.   Zuckerman   M,   Kuhlman   DM,  Thornquist   M,Kiers H.  Five (or three)  robust questionnaire scalefactors of  personality without culture.  Pers In-

    dividual Diff.  1991;12:929-941.15.   Costa  PT Jr,   McCrae  RR. The NEO Personality

    Inventory  Manual.  Odessa,   Fla:   PsychologicalAssessment  Resources;   1985.

    16.   Gray JA.   The Neuropsychology ofAnxiety.  Ne wYork,   NY:  Oxford University  Press;   1982.

    17.   Gray JA.  Anxiety,  personality and the brain.   In:Gale A,   Edwards JA ,   eds.  Physiological Corre-lates of Human Behavior,  III: Individual Differ-ences and Psychopathology. Orlando,  Fla: Aca-demic  Press   Inc:   1983:31-43.

    18.   Cloninger  CR.  A  systematic method for clinicaldescription and classification of personality vari-ants.   Arch   Gen  Psychiatry.   1987;44:573-588.

    19.  CloningerCR.Neurogeneticadaptivemechanismsin   alcoholism.   Science.   1987;236:410-416.

    20.   Eaves   L,  Eysenck   HJ.   The  nature   of extraver-sion:   a genetical analysis.  J Pers Soc  Psychol.1975;32:102-112.

    21.   Heath   AC,   Eaves   LJ,   Martin   NG.   The   geneticstructure   of  personality,   Ill:  multivariate   ge-netic   item analysis  of  the  EPQ  scales. Pers In-dividual Diff.  1989;10:877-888.

    22.   Heath AC,  Cloninger CR,  Martin  NG.  Testing   amodel for  the  genetic  structure of  personality.J Pers  Soc  Psychol.   In   press.

    23.   CloningerCR,   Svrakic DM,  Przybeck   TR.   The   Tri-dimensional Personality Questionnaire: US  nor-mative data. Psychol Rep. 1991;69:1047-1057.

    24.

      Cloninger CR, Sigvardsson  S, Bohman M.  Child-

    hood personality predicts alcohol abuse in  youngadults. Alcoholism.   1988;12:494-505.

    25.   Svrakic DM, Whitehead C, Przybeck TR, CloningerCR.  Differential diagnosis  of   personality  disor-ders   by   the  seven-factor   model of   tempera-ment and  character. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50:991-999.

    26.   Allport GW.  Personality: A Psychological Inter-pretation.  New  York,  NY:  Holt Rinehart  &  Win-ston;   1937.

    27.   Thorpe  WH.  Learning and  Instinct in  Animals.Cambridge,   Mass:  Harvard   University   Press;1956.

    28. Bachevalier J. Ontogenetic development of habit

    and   memory formation   in   primates.   Ann  N   YAcad Sci.   1990;608:457-477.29.   Bachevalier J,   Brickson  M,  Hagger C,  Mishkin

    M. Age  and   sex differences  in  the effects of  se-lective  temporal   lobe   lesion   on   the  formationof visual  discrimination  habits  in  rhesus   mon-

    keys.   Behav Neurosci.   1990;104:885-899.30.   Phillips  RR,  Malamut  BL ,   Bachevalier J,  Mish-

    kin  M.  Dissociation of the effects of  inferior tem-

    poral  a nd   limbic  lesions   on  object discrimina-tion   learning   with   24  hour intertrial   intervals.Behav Brain Res.   1988;27:99-107.

    31.   Squire  LR,  Zola-Morgan   S.  Th e   medial tempo-ral   lobe  memory  system.   Science.  1991;253:1380-1386.

    32. Graf P, Schacter DL.   Implicit and explicit memory

    for new association in normal and  amnesic sub-

     jects.  J Exp  Psychol Learn Mem  Cogn.   1985;11:501-518.

    33. Parkin AJ ,   Reid TK,   Russo  R.  O n  the  differen-tial nature of implicit and explicit memory. MemCogn.   1990;18:507-514.

    34.   Hashtroudi S,  Ferguson SA,  Rappold VA,  Chros-niak   LD.   Data-driven   and   conceptually  driven

    processes in  partial word identification and   rec-ognition. J Exp  Psychol.   1988;14:749-757.

    35.   Schacter  DL,  Rapscak  SZ,   Rubens AB , TharanM,   Laguna J.   Priming   effects   in   a letter by   let-ter reader depend upon  access to the word formsystem.   Neuropsychologia.   1990;28:1079\x=req-\1094.

    36.   Zola-Morgan S,  Squire  LR,  Amaral  D G.  Humanamnesia  and the  medial   temporal   region:   en-during   memory   impairment   following   a   bilat-eral   lesion   limited   to   field   CA1   of   the  hippo-campus.  J Neurosci.  1986;6:2950-2960.

    37.   Malamut  BL,  Saunders   RC,   Mishkin   M.  Mon-

    keys with combined amygdalo-hippocampal le-sions succeed  in object discrimination  learningdespite 24-hour intertrial   intervals. Behav Neu-rosci.   1984;98:759-769.

    38.   Martin  I.  Human classical conditioning.  In : GaleA,   Edwards   JA,   eds.  Physiological  Correlatesof Human  Behavior,   II:  Attention  and Perfor-mance. Orlando,  Fla: Academic Press Inc;  1983:129-148.

    39.   Sigvardsson S,  Bohman M,  Cloninger CR.  Struc-ture and stability of  childhood personality:  pre-diction of later social   adjustment. J  Child Psy-chol Psychiatry.  1987;28:929-946.

    40.   Cloninger CR.  A  unified biosocial  theory of per-sonality and  its  role in the development of  anxi-ety   states.  Psychiatr Dev.  1986;3:167-226.

    41.   Cloninger   CR.  Brain   networks   underlying  per-sonality development.   In:  Carroll  BJ,  Barrett JE,eds.

     Psychopathology and the Brain. New

     York,NY:   Raven   Press;  1991:183-208.42.   Simon HA.  The  architecture of complexity.  Proc

    Am  Philos   Soc.  1962;106:467-482.43.   Cloninger CR,  Gilligan  SB.  Neurogenetic mecha-

    nisms  of  learning:   a  phylogenetic perspective.J Psychiatr  Res.  1987;21:457-472.

    44.   Nixon   SJ,  Parsons   OA.   Cloninger's   tridimen-sional   theory  of   personality:  construct  validityin   a sample   of  college   students.   Pers   Indi-vidual Diff.   1989;10:1261-1267.

    45.   Koehler W ;   Winter   E,   trans.   The  Mentality ofApes.  London, England: Routledge &  Kegan Paul;1927.

    46.   Costa   PT Jr,  McCrae   RR.  The  NEO  personality

    inventory.   In:   Briggs   SR,   Cheek   J,   eds. Per-sonality Measures. Greenwich,  Conn: JAI Press.In  press.

    47.   Waller NG,  Lilienfeld SO, Tellegen A,  Lykken  DT.The  Tridimensional   Personality  Questionnaire:structural validity and comparison with the  Mul-tidimensional  Personality  Questionnaire.   Mul-tivariate Behav  Res.  1991;26:1-23.

    48.   Svrakic DM, Przybeck TR , Cloninger  CR.  Moodstates   andpersonality traits. J   Affect Dis. 1992;24:217-220.

    49. Wetzel RD,   Knesevich MA ,  Brown SL,  Wolff HA,Horn CJ,  Cloninger CR.  Correlates  of  Tridimen-sional Personality Questionnaire Scales with se-lected   Minnesota  Multiphasic  Personality   In-

    ventory   Scales.   Psychol Rep.   In  press.

    wnloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Virginia User on 07/27/2012

  • 8/20/2019 Psychobiological Model of Temperament and Character (1)

    16/16

    50.   Taylor C,  Combs AW . Self-acceptance and ad- justment. J  Consult Psychol.   1953;16:89-91.

    51.   Pekala RJ,  Wenger CF, Levine RL . Individual dif-ferences in phenomenological experience: statesof consciousness  as   a  function   of  absorption.J Pers Soc  Psychol.   1985;48:125-132.

    52.   Goleman  D.   The Meditative Mind:  The  VarietiesofMeditative Experience.  Los  Angeles, Calif: JPTarcher   Inc;  1988.

    53.   Maslow AH.  Motivation and Personality. 2nd  ed.

    New York,   NY:  Harper   &   Row   Publishers   Inc;1970.54.   Maslow AH.   The Farther Reaches of Human Na-

    ture.   New  York,   NY:   The  Viking   Press;   1971.55.   Watson  D L,  Tharp  RG.  Self-Directed Behavior:

    Self-Modification for Personal Adjustment.  5thed.  Pacific  Grove,   Calif:   Brooks/Cole   Publish-ing;   1989.

    56. Leach ER.  Humanism.   In:  Gregory  RL,  ed.   Ox-ford Companion to  the  Mind.  Oxford,  England:Oxford University   Press;   1987:317-319.

    57.   Adler  A.   Individual  psychology.   In:   MurchisonC, ed.  Psychologies of  1930. Worcester,  Mass:Clark University  Press;   1930.

    58.   Coopersmith   S.   The  Antecedents   of   Self\x=req-\

    Esteem.  San  Francisco, Calif: WH Freeman;  1967.59.   Parker G.  The parental bonding  instrument: psy-chometric  properties  reviewed.   Psychiatr Dev.1989;7:317-335.

    60.   Rotter  JB. Generalized   expectancies  for   inter-nal   versus  external   locus   of   control   of rein-

    forcement.  Psychol  Monogr  Gen Appl.   1966;80(Whole   No.  609).

    61.   Lefcourt HM.  Recent developments  in  the  studyof   locus of  control.  Prog Exp  Pers   Res.  1972;6:1-39.

    62.   Frankl   VE.   Man's  Search   for Meaning: An In-troduction  to   Logotherapy.  3rd   ed.  New York,NY:   Simon   &   Schuster   Inc;   1984.

    63.   Crumbaugh   JC.  Cross-validation   of   purpose-

    in-life test  based   on  Frankl's  concepts.  J  Indi-vidual Psychol.   1968;24:74-81.64.   Rogers   CR;  Kirschenbaum   H,   Henderson   VL,

    eds.   The   Carl Rogers   Reader.   Boston,   Mass:Houghton Mifflin]  Co;   1989.

    65.   Covey SR.   The Seven Habits of Highly EffectivePeople: Restoring the  Character Ethic. New  York,

    NY:  Simon   &   Schuster   Inc;   1989.66.   Bandura   A.  Self-efficacy   mechanisms   in   hu-

    man agency.   Am  Psychol.   1982;37:122-147.67.   Bandura A,  Cervone  D.  Self-evaluative and  self-

    efficacy  mechanisms  governing   the   motiva-tional effects of goal systems. J Pers Social Psy-chol.  1983;45:1017-1028.

    68.   Yuasa Y; Nagatomo S,  Kasulis TP, trans. Editor'sintroduction.   In:  Kasulis TP,  ed.   The Body:  To-ward an Eastern Mind-Body Theory. Albany, NY:

    SUNY  Press;   1987:1-15.69.   Berger EM. The relation  between expressed  ac-ceptance of self and expressed acceptance of oth-ers. JAbnorm Social Psychol. 1952;47:778-782.

    70.   Combs   AW,   Snygg   D.   Individual  Behavior:   APerceptual Approach  to  Behavior.  New  York, NY:Harper   &   Row  Publishers   Inc;   1959.

    71.   Erikson  E.  Childhood and Society. 2nd  ed .  NewYork,   NY:  Norton   Publishing;   1963.

    72.   Adler   A.   Social   Interest.   New  York,   NY:   GPPutnam's  Sons;   1939.

    73.   Huxley  A.   The  Perennial Philosophy.   London,England:  Chatto   &  Windus;   1946.

    74.   Kohlberg   L.  The  development of  children's ori-entations  toward  a  moral  order,   I:  sequence  in

    the   development   of moral   thought.   Vita   Hu-mana.   1963;6:11-33.75.   Kohlberg   L.   The   cognitive-developmental   ap-

    proach to  socialization.   In:  Goslin DA,  ed.  Hand-book of Socialization Theory and Research. Chi-cago,   III:   Rand  McNally;  1969.

    76.   Baruk   H.  Patients Are  People  Like  Us:   The Ex-periences of  Half a Century  in  Neuropsychia-try. Ne w York,  NY:  William Morrow &  Co;  1978.

    77.   Princeton   Religion   Research Center.  Religionin America.  Princeton,  NJ:  The Gallup Poll;  1982.

    78.   Koenig HG,  Kvale JN,   Ferrel  C.  Religion and  Well-being   in   later   life.  Gerontologist  1988;28:18\x=req-\28.

    79.   Underhill   E.  Mysticism:  A   Study  in the  Nature

    and Development ofMan's Spiritual Conscious-ness. London, England: Methuen &  Co  Ltd;  1911.80.   White   J.   What Is  Enlightment? Exploring   the

    Goal of the  Spiritual  Path.   Los Angeles,   Calif:JP   Tarcher; 1985.

    81.   Wilber   K.   No  Boundary:  Eastern and   WesternApproaches to Personal Growth. Boston,  Mass:

    Shambhala Publishing   Inc;   1985.82.   Watts   A.  Psychotherapy East and   West   New

    York,   NY: Vintage  Books;   1961.83.   Shaku  S;  Suzuki  DT, trans. Zen  for Americans.

    LaSalle,   Ill:  Open  Court;   1906.84.   Jung  CG.  Modern Man in Search ofa Soul. Lon-

    don,  England:  Routledge   &  Kegan  Paul;   1933.85.   SAS   Institute.   SAS/STAT   User's   Guide,   Re-

    lease 6.03.   Cary,   NC:   SAS   Institute;   1989.86. Erikson E.  Identity:  Youth and Crisis. Ne w York,

    NY:   Norton   Publishing;   1968.87.   Bowlby J.  Attachment and Loss,  I I: Separation,Anger,   and  Loss.   New  York,   NY:   Basic   BooksInc  Publishers;  1973.

    88.   Tellegen   A,   Lykken   DT,   Bouchard   TJ,  WilcoxKJ,   Segal   N,   Rich   S.   Personality   similarity   intwins   reared  apart  a nd together.   J Pers SocialPsychol. 1988;54:1031-1039.

    89.   Benedict   R.   Patterns of Culture.   Boston,   NY:

    Houghton   Mifflin  Co;   1961.90.   Lee   D.   Freedom   and  Culture.   New  York,   NY:

    Spectrum Books,  Prentice-Hall  International Inc;1959.

    91.   Rutter M.  Family and school influences  on  cog-nitive  development.   J  Child  Psychol  Psychia-

    try.   1985;26:683-704.92.   Liebowitz   MR.   In  discussion:  Cloninger   CR.  Aunified biosocial   theory  of  personality  and itsrole  in  the  development of anxiety states.  Psy-chiatr  Dev.   1988;4:377-394.

    93. Sheard   MH,   Marini  SL ,  Bridges  CI, Wagner   E.The  effect of  lithium   on   impulsive   aggressivebehavior   in   man.  Am J  Psychiatry.   1976;133:1409-1413.

    94.   Assagioli  R.  Psychosynthesis: A Manual of Prin-ciples  and   Techniques.   New York,   NY:  VikingPress;   1965.

    95.   Ellis A.  Reason and Emotion in   Psychotherapy.New  York,   NY:   Lyle  Stuart;   1962.

    96.   Musen   G.  Effects   of verbal   labeling   and   expo-

    sure duration on  implicit memory for visual pat-terns. J Exp  Psychol.  1991;17:954-962.97.   Grafton   ST,  Woods   RP,   Mazziotta  JC ,  Phelps

    ME. Somatotopic  mapping   of the  primary   mo-tor  cortex   in   humans:  activation studies  with

    cerebral blood flow and  positron   emission to-

    mography.  J  Neurophysiol.  1991;66:735-743.


Recommended