+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Sandra A. Zerkle & Jennifer E. Arnoldszerkle.web.unc.edu/files/2015/03/cunyposter2018_final.pdf ·...

Sandra A. Zerkle & Jennifer E. Arnoldszerkle.web.unc.edu/files/2015/03/cunyposter2018_final.pdf ·...

Date post: 02-Oct-2020
Category:
Upload: others
View: 0 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
1
Main Question: Are experimental tasks a good measure of next-mention likelihood? 1) Metacognitive judgments 2) Story continuations Results Sandra A. Zerkle & Jennifer E. Arnold Discourse constraints on referential predictability: Is the subject predictable? Background The grammatical subject of a sentence is a topical position (Ariel, 1990; Givón, 1983; Grosz et al., 1995), and this constrains how we produce pronouns (Fukumura & van Gompel, 2010; Kehler et al., 2008; Stevenson et al. 1994) and interpret pronouns (Gernsbacher, 1990; Gordon et al., 1993). Can topicality be defined in terms of predictability? Do people have an expectation for the subject to be mentioned again in natural discourses and experimental tasks? Support for predictability measures of frequency in real discourse show that subjects are mentioned more often than objects/obliques: Evidence against predictability experimental tasks measuring next-mention likelihood: People tend to talk about the cause . People do not tend to talk about the subject if it isn’t the cause. Ariel, M. (1990). Accessing Noun-Phrase Antecedents. v Arnold, J. E. (1998). Reference form and discourse patterns (Doctoral dissertation, Stanford University).v Arnold, J. E. (2001). The effect of thematic role on pronoun use and frequency of reference continuation. Discourse Processes. v Brocher, A., Chiriacescu, S., & von Heusinger, K. (2016). Effects of information status... Discourse Processes. v Frank, M., & Goodman, N. (2012). Predicting pragmatic reasoning in language games. Science. v Fukumura, K., & Van Gompel, R. (2010). Choosing anaphoric expressions: Do people take into account likelihood of reference? JML. v Gernsbacher, M. A. (2013). Language comprehension as structure building. v Givón, T. (1983). Topic continuity in discourse: A quantitative cross-language study (Vol. 3). v Gordon, P., Grosz, B., & Gilliom, L. (1993). Pronouns, names, and the centering of attention in discourse. Cognitive Science. v Grosz, B., Weinstein, S., & Joshi, A. (1995). Centering: A framework for modeling the local coherence of discourse. Comp. linguistics. v Kehler, A., Kertz, L., Rohde, H., & Elman, J. (2008). Coherence and coreference revisited. Journal of semantics. v Kehler, A., & Rohde, H. (2013). A probabilistic reconciliation of coherence-driven & centering-driven theories... Theoretical Linguistics. v Nappa, R., & Arnold, J. E. (2014). The road to understanding is paved with the speaker’s intentions... Cognitive psychology. v Rohde, H., & Kehler, A. (2014). Grammatical and information-structural influences on pronoun production. LCN. v Stevenson, R. J., Crawley, R. A., & Kleinman, D. (1994). Thematic roles, focus and the representation of events. LCP. Funded by NSF Grant 1651000 to J. Arnold Contact: [email protected] 65% 74% 0% 20% 40% 60% 80% 100% 1-sentence 3-sentence % subject mentioned first Exp. 4: past tense/PP phrase Subjects ARE predictable 81% 35% 10% 0% 20% 40% 60% 80% 100% Gaze to Subject Gaze Neutral Gaze to Non-Sub % subject chosen next Exp. 1: metacognitive judgments Subjects NOT predictable 65% 50% 26% 66% 61% 44% 0% 20% 40% 60% 80% 100% Gaze to Subject Gaze Neutral Gaze to Non-Sub % subject mentioned first Exp. 2: story continuation Subjects NOT predictable 1-sentence 3-sentence Methods : all participants native English M-Turk workers Experiments 1 & 2 (N=67, N= 91) Fragments of video stimuli (Nappa & Arnold, 2014), ‘X with Y’ construction Narrator: “This story is about Panda Bear and Puppy. This is Panda Bear (places animal), and this is Puppy (places animal). Panda Bear is having lunch with Puppy. Narrator gazed when uttering pronoun: Subject (A), Neutral (B), Non-Subject (C) Here, video ends after gaze but before pronoun Exp. 1: next mention preference: “Who do you think she will talk about next? Exp. 2: story continuation: “Type in a continuation to the story 2A: one sentence long; 2B: three sentences long Experiments 3 & 4 (N=22, N=27) Written stimuli (no manipulation of gaze); both experiments tested story continuation, with length (1- vs. 3-sentence) manipulated by block Exp. 3: present tense: “Ana is cleaning up with Liz.” Exp. 4: past tense, PP phrase: “Ana was cleaning up with Liz in the kitchen.” 32% 55% 0% 20% 40% 60% 80% 100% 1-sentence 3-sentence % subject mentioned first Exp. 3: present tense Subjects NOT predictable 42% 10% 0% 20% 40% 60% 80% 100% subject referents obj-of-PP referents % of all first references Arnold (2001): corpus analysis 73% 27% 0% 20% 40% 60% 80% 100% Subject Object/Oblique % total references Arnold (1998): text analyses Q2: Does gaze affect predictability judgments? Q3: Does the experimental task change performance? Conclusions Subject-mention in experiments only matches corpus frequency in past- tense narratives è Experimental tasks are NOT a good measure of next-mention predictability In metacognitive & story continuation tasks, there may be an expectation of “fairness”: Panda Bear was the topic of the last sentence, so now Puppy should be mentioned. Task demands modulate performance: More subjects mentioned in longer discourse (Brocher et al., 2016) More subjects mentioned in past tense/PP phrase constructions Raises questions about the use of simple experimental tasks to estimate the probability of reference (e.g., Frank & Goodman, 2012) Experiments 1 & 2: Subject and gaze predictability Experiments 3 & 4: Subject predictability in short vs. long continuations Are subject characters judged to be more predictable? NO Are gazed-at characters judged to be more predictable? YES p<.0001 Are subject characters more likely to be mentioned next? NO Are gazed-at characters more likely to be mentioned next? YES p=0.002 Are subject characters more likely to be mentioned in longer continuations? YES p=0.0068 Are subject characters more likely to be mentioned in longer continuations? YES p=0.028 Numerical trend: subjects more likely in longer narratives Are subject characters more likely to be mentioned next? NO Are subject characters more likely to be mentioned next? YES 73% 26% 54% 0% 20% 40% 60% 80% 100% Subj-biased IC Obj-biased IC Non-IC % subject continuations Rohde & Kehler (2014): sentence continuations (no pronoun prompt)
Transcript
Page 1: Sandra A. Zerkle & Jennifer E. Arnoldszerkle.web.unc.edu/files/2015/03/cunyposter2018_final.pdf · Main Question: Are experimental tasks a good measure of next-mention likelihood?

MainQuestion:Areexperimentaltasksagoodmeasureofnext-mentionlikelihood?1)  Metacognitivejudgments2)  Storycontinuations

Results

SandraA.Zerkle&JenniferE.Arnold

Discourseconstraintsonreferentialpredictability:Isthesubjectpredictable?

Background•  Thegrammaticalsubjectofasentenceisatopicalposition(Ariel,1990;Givón,1983;Groszetal.,1995),andthisconstrainshowweproducepronouns(Fukumura&vanGompel,2010;Kehleretal.,2008;Stevensonetal.1994)andinterpretpronouns(Gernsbacher,1990;Gordonetal.,1993).

•  Cantopicalitybedefinedintermsofpredictability?Dopeoplehaveanexpectationforthesubjecttobementionedagaininnaturaldiscoursesandexperimentaltasks?

Supportforpredictability–measuresoffrequencyinrealdiscourseshowthatsubjectsarementionedmoreoftenthanobjects/obliques:

Evidenceagainstpredictability– experimentaltasksmeasuringnext-mentionlikelihood:•  Peopletendtotalkaboutthecause.

•  Peopledonottendtotalkaboutthesubjectifitisn’tthecause.

Ariel,M.(1990).AccessingNoun-PhraseAntecedents.vArnold,J.E.(1998).Referenceformanddiscoursepatterns(Doctoraldissertation,StanfordUniversity).v Arnold,J.E.(2001).Theeffectofthematicroleonpronounuseandfrequencyofreferencecontinuation.DiscourseProcesses.v Brocher,A.,Chiriacescu,S.,&vonHeusinger,K.(2016).Effectsofinformationstatus...DiscourseProcesses.v Frank,M.,&Goodman,N.(2012).Predictingpragmaticreasoninginlanguagegames.Science.v Fukumura,K.,&VanGompel,R.(2010).Choosinganaphoricexpressions:Dopeopletakeintoaccountlikelihoodofreference?JML.v Gernsbacher,M.A.(2013).Languagecomprehensionasstructurebuilding.v Givón,T.(1983).Topiccontinuityindiscourse:Aquantitativecross-languagestudy(Vol.3).v Gordon,P.,Grosz,B.,&Gilliom,L.(1993).Pronouns,names,andthecenteringofattentionindiscourse.CognitiveScience.v Grosz,B.,Weinstein,S.,&Joshi,A.(1995).Centering:Aframeworkformodelingthelocalcoherenceofdiscourse.Comp.linguistics.v Kehler,A.,Kertz,L.,Rohde,H.,&Elman,J.(2008).Coherenceandcoreferencerevisited.Journalofsemantics.v Kehler,A.,&Rohde,H.(2013).Aprobabilisticreconciliationofcoherence-driven&centering-driventheories...TheoreticalLinguistics.v Nappa,R.,&Arnold,J.E.(2014).Theroadtounderstandingispavedwiththespeaker’sintentions...Cognitivepsychology.v Rohde,H.,&Kehler,A.(2014).Grammaticalandinformation-structuralinfluencesonpronounproduction.LCN.v Stevenson,R.J.,Crawley,R.A.,&Kleinman,D.(1994).Thematicroles,focusandtherepresentationofevents.LCP.

FundedbyNSFGrant1651000toJ.Arnold

Contact:[email protected]

65%74%

0%20%40%60%80%

100%

1-sentence 3-sentence

%su

bjectm

entio

nedfirst Exp.4:pasttense/PPphrase

SubjectsAREpredictable

81%

35%

10%0%

20%

40%

60%

80%

100%

GazetoSubject GazeNeutral GazetoNon-Sub

%su

bjectcho

senne

xt

Exp.1:metacognitivejudgmentsSubjectsNOTpredictable

65%50%

26%

66% 61%44%

0%

20%

40%

60%

80%

100%

GazetoSubject GazeNeutral GazetoNon-Sub

%su

bjectm

entio

nedfirst

Exp.2:storycontinuationSubjectsNOTpredictable

1-sentence3-sentence

Methods:allparticipantsnativeEnglishM-TurkworkersExperiments1&2(N=67,N=91)•  Fragmentsofvideostimuli(Nappa&Arnold,2014),‘XwithY’construction•  Narrator:“ThisstoryisaboutPandaBearandPuppy.ThisisPandaBear

(placesanimal),andthisisPuppy(placesanimal).PandaBearishavinglunchwithPuppy.”

•  Narratorgazedwhenutteringpronoun:Subject(A),Neutral(B),Non-Subject(C)•  Here,videoendsaftergazebutbeforepronoun•  Exp.1:nextmentionpreference:“Whodoyouthinkshewilltalkaboutnext?”•  Exp.2:storycontinuation:“Typeinacontinuationtothestory”•  2A:onesentencelong;2B:threesentenceslong

Experiments3&4(N=22,N=27)•  Writtenstimuli(nomanipulationofgaze);bothexperimentstestedstory

continuation,withlength(1-vs.3-sentence)manipulatedbyblock•  Exp.3:presenttense:“AnaiscleaningupwithLiz.”•  Exp.4:pasttense,PPphrase:“AnawascleaningupwithLizinthekitchen.”

32%

55%

0%20%40%60%80%

100%

1-sentence 3-sentence%su

bjectm

entio

nedfirst

Exp.3:presenttenseSubjectsNOTpredictable

42%

10%

0%

20%

40%

60%

80%

100%

subjectreferents obj-of-PPreferents

%ofa

llfirstre

ferences Arnold(2001):corpusanalysis

73%

27%

0%

20%

40%

60%

80%

100%

Subject Object/Oblique

%to

talreferen

ces

Arnold(1998):textanalyses

Q2:Doesgazeaffectpredictabilityjudgments?

Q3:Doestheexperimentaltaskchangeperformance?

Conclusions•  Subject-mentioninexperimentsonlymatchescorpusfrequencyinpast-tensenarratives

•  èExperimentaltasksareNOTagoodmeasureofnext-mentionpredictability

•  Inmetacognitive&storycontinuationtasks,theremaybeanexpectationof“fairness”:PandaBearwasthetopicofthelastsentence,sonowPuppyshouldbementioned.

•  Taskdemandsmodulateperformance:•  Moresubjectsmentionedinlongerdiscourse(Brocheretal.,2016)•  Moresubjectsmentionedinpasttense/PPphraseconstructions

•  Raisesquestionsabouttheuseofsimpleexperimentaltaskstoestimatetheprobabilityofreference(e.g.,Frank&Goodman,2012)

Experiments1&2:Subjectandgazepredictability

Experiments3&4:Subjectpredictabilityinshortvs.longcontinuations

Aresubjectcharactersjudged

tobemorepredictable?

NO

Aregazed-atcharactersjudged

tobemorepredictable?

YESp<.0001

Aresubjectcharactersmore

likelytobementionednext?

NO

Aregazed-atcharactersmore

likelytobementionednext?

YESp=0.002

Aresubjectcharactersmore

likelytobementionedinlonger

continuations?

YESp=0.0068

Aresubjectcharactersmore

likelytobementionedinlonger

continuations?

YESp=0.028

Numericaltrend:subjectsmorelikelyinlongernarratives

Aresubjectcharactersmore

likelytobementionednext?

NO

Aresubjectcharactersmore

likelytobementionednext?

YES

73%

26%

54%

0%20%40%60%80%

100%

Subj-biasedIC Obj-biasedIC Non-IC

%su

bjectcon

tinua

tions

Rohde&Kehler(2014):sentencecontinuations(nopronounprompt)

Recommended