+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Specialist foragers in forest bee communities are small ... · 1. Individual pollinators that...

Specialist foragers in forest bee communities are small ... · 1. Individual pollinators that...

Date post: 22-Oct-2020
Category:
Upload: others
View: 1 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
31
1158 | wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/jane J Anim Ecol. 2019;88:1158–1167. © 2019 The Authors. Journal of Animal Ecology © 2019 British Ecological Society 1 | INTRODUCTION Pollinators’ interactions with plants tend to be generalized when viewed at the species level, with most pollinator species collect- ing floral resources from multiple plant families (Waser, Chittka, Price, Williams, & Ollerton, 1996). Although such a species-level understanding of plant–pollinator interactions yields important insights about communities, for example with respect to their sta- bility (Bascompte, Jordano, & Olesen, 2006) and restoration (Kaiser- Bunbury et al., 2017), it also obscures the biology of individual-level interactions, which can strongly affect patterns of pollen transfer between plants (Brosi, 2016). Foraging bout specialization, or the specialization of an individual pollinator within a foraging bout (i.e. a single foraging trip), affects plant reproductive success because individuals that move sequentially between plants of the same spe- cies transfer more conspecific pollen and less heterospecific pollen between flowers (Campbell & Motten, 1985; Morales & Traveset, 2008). Here, we use the term foraging bout specialization to refer to patterns of both random foraging (e.g. when an individual happens to specialize, because there is only one rewarding plant species in its foraging path) and non-random foraging (i.e. floral constancy, when an individual specializes despite there being multiple rewarding Received: 5 November 2018 | Accepted: 2 April 2019 DOI: 10.1111/1365-2656.13003 RESEARCH ARTICLE Specialist foragers in forest bee communities are small, social or emerge early Colleen Smith 1,2 | Lucia Weinman 1,2 | Jason Gibbs 3 | Rachael Winfree 2 1 Graduate Program in Ecology & Evolution, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, New Jersey 2 Department of Ecology, Evolution, and Natural Resources, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, New Jersey 3 Department of Entomology, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Manitoba, Canada Correspondence Colleen Smith Email: [email protected] Funding information Xerces Society for Invertebrate Conservation; Natural Resources Conservation Service; Garden Club of America Handling Editor: Julian Resasco Abstract 1. Individual pollinators that specialize on one plant species within a foraging bout transfer more conspecific and less heterospecific pollen, positively affecting plant reproduction. However, we know much less about pollinator specialization at the scale of a foraging bout compared to specialization by pollinator species. 2. In this study, we measured the diversity of pollen carried by individual bees forag- ing in forest plant communities in the mid-Atlantic United States. 3. We found that individuals frequently carried low-diversity pollen loads, suggest- ing that specialization at the scale of the foraging bout is common. Individuals of solitary bee species carried higher diversity pollen loads than did individuals of social bee species; the latter have been better studied with respect to foraging bout specialization, but account for a small minority of the world’s bee species. Bee body size was positively correlated with pollen load diversity, and individuals of polylectic (but not oligolectic) species carried increasingly diverse pollen loads as the season progressed, likely reflecting an increase in the diversity of flowers in bloom. Furthermore, the seasonal increase in pollen load diversity was stronger for bees visiting trees and shrubs than for bees visiting herbaceous plants. 4. Overall, our results showed that both plant and pollinator species’ traits as well as community-level patterns of flowering phenology are likely to be important determinants of individual-level interactions in plant–pollinator communities. KEYWORDS floral constancy, foraging behaviour, individuals, phenology, pollinator
Transcript
  • 1158  |  wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/jane J Anim Ecol. 2019;88:1158–1167.© 2019 The Authors. Journal of Animal Ecology © 2019 British Ecological Society

    1  | INTRODUCTION

    Pollinators’ interactions with plants tend to be generalized when viewed at the species level, with most pollinator species collect-ing floral resources from multiple plant families (Waser, Chittka, Price, Williams, & Ollerton, 1996). Although such a species-level understanding of plant–pollinator interactions yields important insights about communities, for example with respect to their sta-bility (Bascompte, Jordano, & Olesen, 2006) and restoration (Kaiser-Bunbury et al., 2017), it also obscures the biology of individual-level interactions, which can strongly affect patterns of pollen transfer

    between plants (Brosi, 2016). Foraging bout specialization, or the specialization of an individual pollinator within a foraging bout (i.e. a single foraging trip), affects plant reproductive success because individuals that move sequentially between plants of the same spe-cies transfer more conspecific pollen and less heterospecific pollen between flowers (Campbell & Motten, 1985; Morales & Traveset, 2008). Here, we use the term foraging bout specialization to refer to patterns of both random foraging (e.g. when an individual happens to specialize, because there is only one rewarding plant species in its foraging path) and non-random foraging (i.e. floral constancy, when an individual specializes despite there being multiple rewarding

    Received: 5 November 2018  |  Accepted: 2 April 2019DOI: 10.1111/1365-2656.13003

    R E S E A R C H A R T I C L E

    Specialist foragers in forest bee communities are small, social or emerge early

    Colleen Smith1,2  | Lucia Weinman1,2 | Jason Gibbs3 |   Rachael Winfree2

    1Graduate Program in Ecology & Evolution, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, New Jersey2Department of Ecology, Evolution, and Natural Resources, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, New Jersey3Department of Entomology, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Manitoba, Canada

    CorrespondenceColleen SmithEmail: [email protected]

    Funding informationXerces Society for Invertebrate Conservation; Natural Resources Conservation Service; Garden Club of America

    Handling Editor: Julian Resasco

    Abstract1. Individual pollinators that specialize on one plant species within a foraging bout

    transfer more conspecific and less heterospecific pollen, positively affecting plant reproduction. However, we know much less about pollinator specialization at the scale of a foraging bout compared to specialization by pollinator species.

    2. In this study, we measured the diversity of pollen carried by individual bees forag-ing in forest plant communities in the mid-Atlantic United States.

    3. We found that individuals frequently carried low-diversity pollen loads, suggest-ing that specialization at the scale of the foraging bout is common. Individuals of solitary bee species carried higher diversity pollen loads than did individuals of social bee species; the latter have been better studied with respect to foraging bout specialization, but account for a small minority of the world’s bee species. Bee body size was positively correlated with pollen load diversity, and individuals of polylectic (but not oligolectic) species carried increasingly diverse pollen loads as the season progressed, likely reflecting an increase in the diversity of flowers in bloom. Furthermore, the seasonal increase in pollen load diversity was stronger for bees visiting trees and shrubs than for bees visiting herbaceous plants.

    4. Overall, our results showed that both plant and pollinator species’ traits as well as community-level patterns of flowering phenology are likely to be important determinants of individual-level interactions in plant–pollinator communities.

    K E Y WO RD S

    floral constancy, foraging behaviour, individuals, phenology, pollinator

  •      |  1159Journal of Animal EcologySMITH eT al.

    plant species in its foraging path), both of which have similar con-sequences for plants in terms of their effects on pollen transfer. Despite the potential functional importance of pollinator foraging bout specialization, little is known about its prevalence across a wide diversity of pollinator species.

    Most of what we know about foraging bout specialization comes from studies of bees in only two of the roughly 440 bee genera: Apis (honeybees) and Bombus (bumblebees). Studies of bees in these genera suggest that foraging bout specialization is quite common. Foraging honeybees have been found to carry pure pollen loads an overwhelming majority (i.e. >90%) of the time, and bumblebees also tend to carry pure pollen loads more often than not, though less fre-quently than honeybees (range 49% to 76% of the time; Free, 1963; Grant, 1950; Leonhardt & Blüthgen, 2012). However, studies of hon-eybees and bumblebees might tell us little about the prevalence of foraging bout specialization in other bee taxa, for two reasons. First, both Apis and Bombus are eusocial, while 90% of bee species are not (Danforth, Cardinal, Praz, Almeida, & Michez, 2013). Second, and more generally, if bee species vary in their foraging behaviour, then studies of just a few bee species might not be very informative for understanding foraging bout specialization across many bee species. In the few pollen load studies of bees from other genera, we see that, in some species, as many as 97% of individuals carry pure pollen loads while foraging (in stingless bees; Ramalho, Giannini, Malagodi-Braga, & Imperatriz-Fonseca, 1994), but in other species, only 34% of individuals carry pure pollen loads (in mason bees; Eckhardt, Haider, Dorn, & Müller, 2014). However, because research has focused on Apis and Bombus, we know very little about how common foraging bout specialization is in bees across a wider diversity of genera and life histories (but see Grant, 1950; Motten, 1986; Pornon, Andalo, Burrus, & Escaravage, 2017).

    The life-history traits of pollinators, and also of the plant spe-cies in their environment, could affect how commonly individuals exhibit foraging bout specialization. First, generalist solitary bee species might be less specialized in their foraging behaviour than social bee species. Social species may have a tendency to specialize while foraging for a number of reasons related to cognitive ability and resource partitioning (Chittka, Thomson, & Waser, 1999; Jones & Agrawal, 2017), and two experiments have shown that individuals of social bees species exhibited foraging bout specialization more often than individuals of solitary bee species; in both studies, the social bees were faster to learn to associate floral rewards with a plant species’ morphology (Amaya-Márgez & Wells, 2008; Dukas & Real, 1991). However, neither study included more than one social or solitary bee species, and there has yet to be a rigorous test of the pattern that social bees are more specialized foragers than solitary ones, where individuals are compared across multiple social and sol-itary bee species foraging in the same environment.

    Next, bee body size, bee dietary breadth and the growth form of the available plants could each affect how specialized a forager will be, because they affect the number of rewarding plant species a foraging bee encounters. First, we expect individuals of large bee species to carry more diverse pollen loads, even if this is by chance

    alone. Large species require more overall pollen for their offspring and can fit a larger volume of pollen in their pollen loads (Müller et al., 2006; Ramalho et al., 1994). They will thus likely visit more individual flowers during a single foraging bout, which would cause them to also encounter more plant species. Second, individuals of generalist bee species (hereafter “polylectic” bees) are able to use pollen from more plant species than individuals of specialist bee spe-cies (hereafter “oligolectic” bees), which are physiologically and be-haviourally limited to only a few plant taxa for pollen (Praz, Müller, & Dorn, 2008; Williams, 2003). Thus, we expect individuals of polylec-tic species to carry more diverse pollen loads than individuals of oligolectic species (Cripps & Rust, 1989; Müller, 1996). Finally, we expect bees that visit herbs to carry more diverse pollen loads than bees that visit trees or shrubs, because the large floral displays pro-duced by trees and shrubs could induce a foraging bee to stay on one plant longer and save the search costs of foraging elsewhere. This should lead to fewer encounters with other plant species (and also an increase in the rate of selfing, a mating cost from the plant's per-spective; Kunin, 1993, Barrett & Harder, 1996, Castilla et al., 2017). Alternatively, each of these traits may matter very little if most bees exhibit floral constancy, a behaviour that increases foraging effi-ciency by removing the cognitive demands associated with switching between plant species (Chittka et al., 1999; Gegear & Laverty, 2005). Because all bees benefit from increased foraging efficiency, floral constancy might be very common, and there might be little variation between individuals for these traits to explain.

    Finally, foraging bout specialization by pollinators could vary over time with seasonal changes in the diversity of plant species in bloom. All regions exhibit seasonal changes in the diversity of floral resources available to pollinators (Ogilvie & Forrest, 2017). In regions with shortened growing seasons, floral diversity tends to exhibit uni-modal or bimodal patterns, with diversity increasing as environmen-tal conditions become more favourable (e.g. Anderson & Hubricht, 1940, Heinrich, 1976, Kochmer & Handel, 1986, Morales, Dodge, & Inouye, 2005, Makrodimos, Blionis, Krigas, & Vokou, 2008, Craine, Wolkovich, Towne, & Kembel, 2012). Studies show that individuals respond to increases in floral diversity by visiting more plant species within a foraging bout (Flanagan, Mitchell, & Karron, 2011; Kunin, 1993; Lucas et al., 2018). However, if some bees (e.g. social bees, small bees, oligolectic bees) have a strong tendency to exhibit for-aging bout specialization, changes in floral diversity over time might have little effect on foraging bout specialization in these species.

    In this study, we measure foraging bout specialization for for-est bee communities native to eastern US temperate deciduous forests. These forests, which once covered much of eastern North America, support diverse native bee communities in which adult bees of many species are active for two to three months, from the start of spring in March through the closing of the forest canopy in May (Harrison, Gibbs, & Winfree, 2018). Forest bees are under-studied with respect to floral resource use and are largely neglected by common pollinator conservation efforts, such as pollinator floral restorations, which tend to only include summer-blooming plants (Hicks et al., 2016; Wood et al., 2018); thus, there is a great research

  • 1160  |    Journal of Animal Ecology SMITH eT al.

    need for understanding the foraging needs of these species. We also chose this system because it provides a natural gradient in flower-ing diversity; only a few species flower in the earliest weeks, but diversity steadily increases over the course of the spring (Anderson & Hubricht, 1940; Heinrich, 1976; Kochmer & Handel, 1986). We measured pollinator foraging patterns in plant–pollinator communi-ties throughout this period by removing and identifying the pollen collected into individual bees’ scopae and corbiculae (pollen-car-rying structures). These pollen loads provide a history of the plant species a female bee has visited for pollen during a single foraging bout, because the bee removes the pollen from her pollen-carrying structures when she returns to her nest after each foraging bout. Pollen loads thus enable the measurement of pollen foraging bout specialization across many individuals and species.

    To better understand the dynamics of pollen foraging across many bee species, we ask the following: (a) What is the distribution of pollen load diversities across individual bees collected while for-aging? (b) Do species-level traits (bee sociality, bee body size, bee dietary breadth or plant growth form) affect the diversity of pollen carried by individuals? and (c) Is there a seasonal gradient in the di-versity of individual pollen loads, and is this mediated by plant or pollinator species-level traits?

    2  | MATERIALS AND METHODS

    2.1 | Study design and data collection

    Temperate deciduous forests support many native bee species, most of which are active as adults only in the spring when the forest canopy has not closed due to leaf-out, and spring ephemeral herbs and animal-pollinated trees are in bloom (March–May in our study system). Our study focused on native bees in the genera Andrena, Augochlora, Augochloropsis, Bombus, Colletes, Lasioglossum and Osmia (hereafter “forest bees”), because these genera are known to include species associated with forest habitat (Harrison et al., 2018), and we wished to better understand floral resource use by this understud-ied group. We excluded all male bees and parasitic species because these bees do not collect pollen (Michener, 2000).

    We collected bees at four forested study sites in New Jersey and Pennsylvania (Table S1). All sites were deciduous forests, located in the Piedmont ecoregion, with oak–hickory or maple–beech–birch overstories and a mix of native and non-native plants in the herba-ceous and shrub layers. Average land cover at a 1 km radius around each site was 55.9% (±12.5% SE) forest, 16.0% suburban (±5.0% SE), 14.9% (± 6.7% SE) agricultural, 1.1% (± 0.9% SE) water and 12.2 (±4.9% SE) wetland matrix habitat.

    We visited sites between 23 March 2016 and 20 May 2016. Sites were visited on separate days by two to three data collectors at a time. All sampling was conducted between 10:00 and 19:00 on sunny days when the temperature was high enough for bees to be active (>14°C), but we otherwise did not standardize sampling between sites. At each site, we walked over an area of approxi-mately 26,390 m2 (±8,875 m2 SE), both through the understorey

    and along forest edges, searching for plants with flowers in bloom. We sampled bees from as many plant species as we could find, 48 plant species in total (mean = 15.0 ± 5.2 SE plant species per site; Table S2). We used an insect net to collect any bee we observed visiting a flower, placing each individual bee into a separate clean vial. Because our primary interest was in the polylectic bee spe-cies whose foraging at the individual level has greater potential to vary, we minimized collecting a highly abundant bee species, Andrena erigeniae Robertson 1891, which is narrowly oligolectic on a single plant species, Claytonia virginica L. We froze all bees in coolers in the field and stored them at −30°C in a laboratory freezer until processing. Bees were identified using published revi-sions (see Gibbs, Ascher, Rightmyer, & Isaacs, 2017 for references; LaBerge, 1986).

    We checked whether floral diversity increased during the course of spring at our study sites by examining how the diversity of plant species we collected bees from changed over time. We found a pos-itive relationship, as expected based on other studies in our region (Figure S1).

    2.2 | Pollen analysis

    In the laboratory, we removed and stained pollen from the pollen-collecting structures (scopae or corbiculae) of all female non-para-sitic forest bees. Because a female bee removes her pollen load after each foraging trip, pollen loads represent a history of the plant spe-cies a bee has visited up to the point it was collected during the cur-rent foraging bout. Additionally, because female bees actively pack pollen into scopae and corbiculae, the pollen load also represents the pollen actually chosen by the foraging female for her offspring's consumption.

    We removed pollen from each bee's scopal structures used two 1-mm3 cubes of fuchsin (Kearns & Inouye, 1993). We dabbed each cube over the scopal hairs or corbiculae of a specimen, including both legs (for bees with scopae or corbiculae on their legs), covering as much surface area as possible. We stopped either when the cube was covered in pollen or there was no pollen left on the specimen. Fuchsin cubes were then melted onto glass slides using a hot plate.

    We identified pollen to the finest taxonomic level possible under a light microscope. Pollen identifications were conducted using a pollen reference library we curated from our study sites. The library contained a total of 127 plant species, including both forest and for-est-edge plant species. Pollen reference libraries were compiled by collecting freshly dehiscent anthers from plants in bloom at a site and staining pollen from these anthers using fuchsin gel. We were not able to distinguish some closely related plant species by their pollen and thus in some cases use genus-, family- or higher-level groupings (Table S3). Lastly, not all pollen in bee pollen loads was in our pollen reference libraries. We used morphospecies classifica-tions for these pollens, but these morphospecies-level pollens con-stituted only 6.5% of all pollen in pollen loads. Hereafter, we refer to all pollen groups (of family, genera, species and morphospecies) as “morphotypes”.

  •      |  1161Journal of Animal EcologySMITH eT al.

    For each bee, we systematically surveyed about half of all pol-len grains that had adhered to the surface of the two fuchsin cubes (thus, we use a standard amount of sampling effort for each bee). We vertically scanned half of each fuchsin stain at 200x magnification and recorded all pollen morphotypes we observed in each scan. We counted the number of scans in which each morphotype occurred to quantify each pollen morphotype's relative incidence in the pollen load. To avoid analysing pollen that might be contamination, we only recorded pollen morphotypes that had at least five pollen grains in a slide. We excluded individual bees from analyses if they carried no detectable pollen (n = 25).

    2.3 | Classifying species’ traits

    We classified bees and plants by the following species-level traits: bee sociality, bee species-level dietary specialization (hereafter, lecty), bee body size and plant growth form (Table S4). To categorize bees by their sociality and lecty, we used a previously compiled traits database of bees in the northeastern United States (Bartomeus et al., 2013). We categorized bees as either solitary or social, and as either oligolectic (pollen specialists) or polylectic (pollen generalists). We consider oligolectic species to be those that only collect pol-len from a single plant family, genus or species. We used the USDA Plants database (USDA & NRCS, 2018) to classify each plant species that we collected a bee from by its growth form as either herbaceous or woody.

    We measured bee body size for the majority of species using a published independent dataset (Cariveau et al., 2016), though for seven species not in the independent dataset and for all species from the genus Bombus, we measured bee body size using specimens from our own data, using the same methods as Cariveau et al. (2016). Queens and workers in the genus Bombus differed markedly in body size, so for this genus we measured the body sizes of queens and workers separately. For between 1 and 10 specimens of each bee species, we measured intertegular distance, the distance between the bee's wing bases. Intertegular distance is strongly correlated with dry body mass, and we used a published equation to convert intertegular distance to body mass (Cane, 1987).

    2.4 | Calculating pollen load diversity

    We measured the diversity of pollen in individual pollen loads using richness.

    We also compared different choices of diversity metrics by re-running our analyses using Hill-Shannon diversity (which provides a balanced measure of rare and common species) and Hill-Simpson diversity (which emphasizes common species) in place of richness (Jost, 2006, Appendix S1: Figure S2).

    2.5 | Analysis methods

    We examined the distribution of pollen load diversities across all individual bees in our study (question i) by plotting histograms of

    pollen load richness. We also plotted separate histograms for each of the nine most common bee species (N ≥ 20) in our data, to exam-ine how these distributions vary between bee species (Appendix S1: Figure S3).

    We used linear mixed-effects models to examine the importance of traits and seasonality to foraging bout specialization, modelling individual pollen load diversity as a function of species’ traits, day-of-year, and the interactions between traits and day-of-year. We started with a global model in which the response variable was the richness of pollen carried by an individual bee. The explanatory vari-ables included four species-level traits: sociality (social or solitary), lecty (oligolectic or polylectic), bee body size, and the growth form of the plant the individual was foraging from when collected (woody or herbaceous), as well as the day-of-year of collection and all two-way interactions between species’ traits and day-of-year. Though three-way interactions might be important for explaining pollen load diversity, we did not include these in our model to avoid model over-fitting.

    We included site and its interaction with day-of-year as addi-tional explanatory variables to account for non-independence of individuals from the same site. To account for non-independence between individuals of the same species, we included species as a random intercept and slope effect in the model, allowing the rela-tionship between day-of-year and pollen load diversity to vary by species (Gelman & Hill, 2007).

    To make model intercepts and interactions interpretable, we standardized and centred continuous explanatory variables (i.e. day-of-year and body size). To account for heteroscedasticity, we loge-transformed pollen load diversity and log10-transformed bee body size. We used the r package “lme4” to fit mixed models to data (Bates, Mächler, Bolker, & Walker, 2015; R Core Team, 2018).

    We used an exploratory method, AICc, to determine the optimal fixed structure of model. We compared the AICc value of the global model with all possible submodels containing the variable day-of-year (using the “dredge” function in the r package “MuMIn”; Bartón, 2017). We picked the model with the lowest AICc value.

    We checked assumptions of normality and homogeneity of vari-ance for the global and best-fitting models using diagnostic plots of model residuals. We checked that explanatory variables did not covary by calculating the variance inflation factor for each fixed effect in the final model. None exceeded three, suggesting there is minimal colinearity between explanatory variables (Zuur, Ieno, Walker, Saveliev, & Smith, 2009). We estimated the best-fitting model's goodness-of-fit by calculating the marginal R2, the variance explained by the fixed effects alone, and the conditional R2, the vari-ance explained by both the fixed and random effects (Bartón, 2017; Johnson, 2014; Nakagawa & Schielzeth, 2013).

    To assess the significance of variables in the final model, we used parametric bootstrapping to obtain 95% confidence intervals around model parameter estimates. Using the R command “boot-Mer,” we generated 1,000 bootstrapped samples from the model and refit the model to these samples to obtain bootstrapped pa-rameter estimates (Bates et al., 2015). We then calculated the

  • 1162  |    Journal of Animal Ecology SMITH eT al.

    2.5 and 97.5 percentile values of the bootstrapped parameter estimates.

    We concluded that a trait affects individual pollen load diversity (question ii) if the trait was included in the best-fitting model, and the bootstrapped confidence intervals around its parameter estimate in that model did not overlap with zero. Because we centred the variable day-of-year around its mean, the main effects of all traits that interact with day-of-year are interpretable as the effect of the trait on pollen load diversity on the mean day of sampling, April 25 (Schielzeth, 2010).

    We concluded that a trait mediated the effect of day-of-year on pollen load diversity (question iii) if the interaction between a trait and day-of-year was included in the best-fitting model and if its bootstrapped confidence intervals around the parameter estimate in that model did not overlap with zero. If there was a significant interaction between day-of-year and a categorical trait, then we ex-amined the confidence intervals around the day-of-year coefficient for each trait value.

    To hedge against the possibility that our results could be due to an arbitrary choice among equally credible models, we also ran the parametric bootstrapping analysis on all models within three AICc values of the best-fitting model (Figure S4).

    3  | RESULTS

    In total, we removed pollen from 441 individuals of 56 forest bee species (Table S5).

    1. What is the distribution of pollen load diversities across indi-vidual bees collected while foraging?

    Across the entire season, 33.4% of individual bees carried pure (i.e. single morphotype) pollen loads (Figure 1). The average richness of a pollen load was 2.28 (±0.07 SE; median = 2). The distribution of pollen load diversities was right-skewed; most individuals carried pollen loads with one or a few pollen morphotypes (Figure 1). Each of the common bee species (N ≥ 20) we examined also had right‐skewed pollen load

    diversity distributions (Figure S3). However, the percentage of individ-uals carrying pure pollen loads varied between bee species, as did the average richness of pollen individuals carried (Appendix S1: Table S6).

    2. Do species-level traits affect the diversity of pollen carried by individuals?

    The best-fitting model of individual pollen load diversity included the main effects of day-of-year, sociality, lecty, body size, plant growth form and site, and the interactions between day-of-year and lecty, between day-of-year and plant growth form, and between day-of-year and body size (Table S7). It explained 38% of variation in individual pollen load diversity (conditional R2 = 0.383), with 24% of the variation explained by the fixed effects alone (marginal R

    2 = 0.243). There were three other models within three AICc values of the best-fitting model (Table S7; Figure S4).

    Individuals of solitary bee species carried significantly higher-diversity pollen loads than individuals of social species (Table 1; Figure 2). Being solitary instead of social was associated with a 28.7% increase in pollen load diversity (when reporting effect sizes, all other variables are held at their reference levels or means). Social bees in the genus Bombus carry very large pollen loads, which might be relatively undersampled. To approximately control for differ-ences between social and solitary bees in pollen load size, we com-pared the pollen load diversities of social and solitary bees within the genus Lasioglossum (N = 90) and found that, directionally, the effect of sociality was the same (Figure S5).

    F I G U R E 1   Distribution of individual pollen load diversities, with diversity measured using richness

    Pro

    port

    ion

    of in

    divi

    dual

    s

    0 2 4 6 8 10

    0.0

    0.1

    0.2

    0.3

    0.4

    0.5

    Pollen load diversity (Richness)

    TA B L E 1   Parameter estimates of the best-fitting model of pollen load richness (loge transformed) and bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals. Significant parameters are highlighted in bold. The model intercept represents the model prediction when categorical variables are held at the reference level and continuous variables are held at their means (reference level = solitary, polylectic bees visiting herbaceous plants at site1). Parameter estimates of categorical variables represent the difference in the model prediction between the non-reference and reference levels. Parameter estimates of continuous variables are interpretable as the change in log(richness) with each standard deviation of the variable. The variable size is log10-transformed

    Parameter Estimate Lower C.I. Upper C.I.

    Intercept 0.765 0.623 0.907

    Day‐of‐year 0.121 0.034 0.201

    Site2 0.207 0.058 0.368

    Site3 0.033 −0.184 0.272

    Site4 −0.041 −0.189 0.104

    Growth −0.060 −0.175 0.056

    Lecty −0.157 −0.446 0.123

    Size 0.169 0.096 0.247

    Sociality −0.252 −0.419 −0.066

    Day‐of‐year:Growth 0.150 0.026 0.276

    Day‐of‐year:Lecty −0.336 −0.572 −0.096

    Day-of-year:Size 0.059 −0.004 0.128

  •      |  1163Journal of Animal EcologySMITH eT al.

    Bee body size was significantly positively associated with pollen load diversity (Figure 2; Table 1), with the largest bees carrying an estimated 130.6% additional morphotypes of pollen in their pollen loads compared to the smallest bees.

    There was no significant difference in the diversity of pollen col-lected by individuals belonging to oligolectic and polylectic species (Table 1). The non-significant difference was driven by seven individ-uals of one oligolectic bee species, Andrena erythronii (results when these seven individuals were removed from the analysis: lecty main effect [C.I.] = −0.35 [−0.67, −0.03]). Individuals from this species car-ried surprisingly diverse pollen loads and only a small percentage of host pollen (Figure 3b; Table S8). The determinations for this bee species were verified using a published key and reference specimens (LaBerge, 1987).

    Plant growth form was included in the best-fitting model, but was not significantly associated with pollen load diversity (Table 1).

    The variable site was also included in the best-fitting model as a main effect. Individuals from different sites varied in the diversity of pollen they carried, from an average of 2.1 to 2.6 pollen morpho-types (Table 1).

    These results were robust to the use of different diversity met-rics (Appendix S1; Figure S2) and to the choice of model (Figure S4).

    3. Is there a seasonal gradient in the diversity of individual pollen loads, and is this mediated by plant or pollinator species-level traits?

    The final model included significant interactions between day-of-year and lecty and between day-of-year and plant growth form (Table 1). An interaction between day-of-year and body size was included in the final model, but it was not significant. The inter-action between day-of-year and sociality was not included in the final model (Table 1).

    Day-of-year had a greater effect on the diversity of pollen carried by polylectic bees than on the diversity of pollen carried by oligolec-tic bees (Table 1; Figure 3). For polylectic bees, the estimated num-ber of pollen morphotypes a bee carried increased by 0.9 from the first day-of-sampling to the last, an increase of 54.2%; for oligolectic

    F I G U R E 2   The effects of body size and sociality on the richness of pollen collected by individual bees. The solid lines show the model predictions, back-transformed from the log scale. For the model prediction, all other variables are held at their reference levels or means

    2.5

    5.0

    7.5

    0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0

    Body mass (mg) (logged)

    Indi

    vidu

    al p

    olle

    n lo

    ad d

    iver

    sity

    SocialitySolitarySocial

    F I G U R E 3   The effect of day-of-year on the richness of pollen collected by individuals of (a) polylectic and (b) oligolectic species. Individuals in the species Andrena erythronii, which carried unusually diverse pollen loads for an oligolectic bee, are highlighted in pink. Lines show model predictions, back-transformed from the log scale. The dashed line is the model prediction for bees visiting herbaceous plants, and the solid line is the model prediction for bees visiting woody plants. For model predictions, all other variables are held at their reference levels or means

    Polylectic Oligolectic

    Apr 01 Apr 15 May 01 May 15 Apr 01 Apr 15 May 01 May 15

    2.5

    5.0

    7.5

    Day of year

    Indi

    vidu

    al p

    olle

    n lo

    ad d

    iver

    sity

    GrowthHerbaceous plantWoody plant

    BeePolylecticOligolecticAndrena erythronii

    (a) (b)

  • 1164  |    Journal of Animal Ecology SMITH eT al.

    bees, the estimated number of pollen morphotypes decreased by 1.5, or 53.6%.

    The final model also included a significant interaction between day-of-year and plant growth form (Table 1). Day-of-year had a greater effect on the pollen load diversities of bees visiting trees and shrubs than on the pollen load diversities of bees visiting her-baceous plants (Table 1; Figure 3). For example, bees visiting woody plants carried an estimated 1.9 additional pollen morphotypes from the first day of sampling to the last, an increase of 163.8%, whereas the diversity of pollen carried by bees visiting herbaceous plants in-creased by only 54.2%.

    We also examined the bootstrapped confidence intervals around the day-of-year coefficient at each trait level and found that the slope of the relationship between day-of-year and pollen load di-versity was significantly positive for polylectic bees visiting herba-ceous plants (slope [C.I.] = 0.12 [0.03, 0.20]) and woody plants (slope [C.I.] = 0.27 [0.17, 0.37]). It was not significantly different from zero for oligolectic bees visiting herbaceous plants (slope [C.I. = −0.21, [−0.45, 0.02]) or woody plants (slope [C.I.] = −0.06 [−0.31, 0.18]).

    We got qualitatively similar results when analysing other diversity metrics, and when analysing other top-ranked models (ΔAICc < 3), with one exception (Figures S2 and S4). The fourth ranked model did not include an interaction between day-of-year and plant growth form, which was significant in the main analysis (Figure S4). It instead included a significant interaction between day-of-year and body size, with day-of-year having a greater effect on the diversity of pollen car-ried by large bees than on the diversity of pollen carried by small bees (Figure S4).

    4  | DISCUSSION

    Pollinator foraging bout specialization affects patterns of pollen transfer among plants and thus could affect plant reproduction. However, most of what we know about it comes from studies of bees in only two genera, Apis and Bombus. Here, we show that, across 56 bee species in seven genera, most individuals carry low-diversity pollen loads, but the percentage of individuals carrying completely pure (i.e. single morphotype) pollen loads is much lower than has been found in studies of Apis and Bombus. In our study, only 34% of all individuals carried pure pollen loads, compared to >90% in studies of Apis and between 49% and 75% in studies of Bombus (Free, 1963; Grant, 1950; Leonhardt & Blüthgen, 2012). Furthermore, we show that foraging bout specialization is affected by plant and pollinator life-history traits, as well as seasonality.

    In our data, social bee species, which have been the focus of most previous studies, carry less diverse pollen loads than solitary bee species, which represent the great majority of bee species on Earth (Figure 2; Table 1). A potential explanation is that social bees have a greater capacity for learning than solitary ones and thus learn more quickly which plant species in their environment is a profitable one to specialize on (Jones & Agrawal, 2017). There is evidence in other animal taxa that sociality and learning capacity are correlated

    (Byrne & Bates, 2007), and in bees, this explanation is supported by two previous studies, both of which found that a social bee spe-cies was faster to learn to associate a plant species’ reward with its morphology and as a result was more likely to exhibit foraging bout specialization (Amaya-Márgez & Wells, 2008; Dukas & Real, 1991). In our study, we also show that social bees are more likely to exhibit foraging bout specialization than solitary ones, but unlike previous research, we show that this pattern exists across individuals from many social and solitary bee species, 21 social bee species in three genera and 35 solitary bee species in five genera.

    Bee body size also affects foraging bout specialization, with small bees being more specialized foragers than large bees. Across the tree of life, body size is one of the major explanatory variables in ecology (Brown, Gillooly, Allen, Savage, & West, 2004). It affects most aspects of an individual's life history and ecology, including metabolism, home range and resource requirements (Brown et al., 2004; Reiss, 1988), all of which are likely to affect foraging bout spe-cialization. A bee that collects more pollen and flies farther distances while foraging (Greenleaf, Williams, Winfree, & Kremen, 2007; Ramalho et al., 1994) will probably encounter more plant species, just by chance. Thus, larger bees would be expected to visit more plant species during a foraging bout, even without presuming they seek out greater pollen diversity. Studies of monospecific crop fields have found that large bees are more effective pollinators than small bees because they deposit more pollen grains onto plant stigmas (Bartomeus, Cariveau, Harrison, & Winfree, 2018; Larsen, Williams, & Kremen, 2005). However, our result suggests that, in diverse wild plant communities, large bees could also transfer a greater propor-tion of heterospecific pollen between flowers, reducing some of their effectiveness as pollinators.

    Foraging bout specialization in polylectic bee species becomes less frequent as spring progresses, most likely due to the wider va-riety of plants blooming later in the season (Anderson & Hubricht, 1940; Heinrich, 1976; Kochmer & Handel, 1986). Floral diversity sets an upper limit on pollen load diversity, so that, as long as generalized foraging is energetically profitable for at least some individuals, it probably tends to increase the average diversity of individual pol-len loads within a community. Many regions with shortened grow-ing seasons exhibit seasonal patterns in floral diversity similar to the one in our study region, with floral diversity increasing to one or two peaks as abiotic conditions become favourable (e.g. Anderson & Hubricht, 1940, Heinrich, 1976, Kochmer & Handel, 1986, Morales et al., 2005, Makrodimos et al., 2008, Craine et al., 2012). As a result, seasonal declines in pollinator foraging bout specialization among polylectic bees, which in many communities comprise the majority of bee species (Minckley & Roulston, 2006), are likely to be common.

    In contrast to polylectic bees, oligolectic bees did not become more generalized in their foraging behaviour over time. With the ex-ception of individuals from one species, they carried mostly or only pollen from their host plants, even in late spring when floral diversity was high (Figure 3b). Surprisingly, oligolectic bees were not different from polylectic bees in the average diversity of pollen they carried (Table 1), but this was primarily due to seven individuals from one

  •      |  1165Journal of Animal EcologySMITH eT al.

    ostensibly oligolectic species, Andrena erythronii, which carried un-usually diverse pollen loads. This species has previously been found to collect non-host pollen when its host plant is not in bloom, so it is likely this species is more flexible in its pollen use than suggested by a typical definition of oligolecty (Michener & Rettenmeyer, 1956). It is also possible we would have observed a stronger effect of lecty had we collected a common oligolectic bee in our system, A. erige-niae, in proportion to its abundance.

    Plant growth form also affected seasonal changes in pollinator foraging bout specialization, with visitors to woody plants being more strongly affected by day-of-year than visitors to herbaceous plants. We expected the opposite, that the large floral displays pro-duced by trees and shrubs would cause pollinators to specialize, even in late spring when floral diversity is high. Though polylectic bees were more specialized foragers on woody plants than on herbaceous ones in early spring, as the spring progressed bee foraging behaviour on woody and herbaceous plants became similar (Figure 3a). A possi-ble explanation is that in early spring, bees are more likely to special-ize on trees, because large floral displays are particularly attractive when flowers in the landscape are otherwise sparse. In late spring, when flowers are more abundant, herbs and trees may be more simi-larly attractive, and foragers may be less likely to specialize on trees.

    Our result has implications for plant fitness and timing of repro-duction because plants blooming later in spring were more likely to be visited by bees that also visited other plant species during the same foraging bout. This could indicate a potential reproductive cost to delaying bloom, via reduced pollination quality (Morales & Traveset, 2008). Other studies have shown that the presence of co-flowering plant species can reduce plant fitness by decreasing pollinator for-aging bout specialization (Campbell & Motten, 1985; Flanagan et al., 2011; Flanagan, Mitchell, Knutowski, & Karron, 2009), and studies show that plants in more diverse regions experience more pollen limitation than plants in less diverse regions (Alonso, Vamosi, Knight, Steets, & Ashman, 2010; Vamosi et al., 2006). Our result emphasizes that temporal differences in floral diversity could also be important. Furthermore, temporal changes in floral diversity have the poten-tial to act as agents of selection on flowering time, because plants can avoid periods of high floral diversity by blooming earlier or later. Future research should investigate to what extent seasonal changes in pollinator foraging bout specialization affect plant fitness and tim-ing of reproduction.

    Our methods of estimating foraging bout specialization imposed two limitations on our study. First, we were not able to identify all pollen to species, and it is possible, for example, that some mono-typic pollen loads actually contained multiple pollen species within the same family or genus. Low taxonomic resolution is a well-known limitation of pollen load data, and most pollen studies combine at least some pollen species by genus or family (e.g. Lopezaraiza-Mikel, Hayes, Whalley, & Memmott, 2007, Tur, Vigalondo, Trøjelsgaard, Olesen, & Traveset, 2014, Keller et al., 2015, Wood et al., 2018). In our data, 67% of all pollen in pollen loads was only resolved to the mor-pho-group (not the species) level (Table S3), though our pollen mor-pho-groups contained few species (mean = 3.8 ± 0.58 SE, median = 3).

    Second, the bees we collected were at all stages of their foraging bouts, and bees at the start of their bout would have visited a smaller number of plants. Though it is possible to obtain pollen loads from bees that have completed entire foraging trips, by removing pollen as bees return to their nests, it is not feasible to do this for a wide array of bee species. These sampling limitations, despite potentially biasing our absolute estimates of foraging bout specialization, apply similarly across bees and therefore should not strongly bias our conclusions regarding the effects of species’ traits and community-level patterns of flowering phenology on foraging bout specialization.

    In conclusion, we show that specialization at the scale of a foraging bout is common across a wide diversity of bee species, but not all bee species are equally likely to exhibit foraging bout specialization at all times. Bee species that are solitary, large or polylectic are less likely to exhibit foraging bout specialization than species that are social, small or oligolectic, either throughout an entire growing season, or when floral diversity peaks. A next step for future research is to investigate how changes in pollinator foraging bout specialization over time and among species affect plant fitness.

    ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

    We thank two anonymous reviewers for providing detailed comments that improved the manuscript. We also thank mem-bers of the Winfree laboratory for helpful comments; Cameron Kanterman and Michael Botros for field and laboratory assistance; and the Xerces Society for Invertebrate Conservation, the Natural Resources Conservation Service, and the Garden Club of America for providing funding.

    AUTHORS’ CONTRIBUTIONS

    C.S. and R.W. conceived the idea for the study. C.S. and L.W. col-lected data. J.G. identified bee specimens. All authors contributed to the writing of the manuscript.

    DATA ACCESSIBILITY

    Data are available from the Dryad Digital Repository: https ://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.2bm34c2 (Smith, Weinman, Gibbs, & Winfree, 2019).

    ORCID

    Colleen Smith https://orcid.org/0000-0003-0702-6927

    REFERENCES

    Alonso, C., Vamosi, J. C., Knight, T. M., Steets, J. A., & Ashman, T. L. (2010). Is reproduction of endemic plant species particularly pollen limited in biodiversity hotspots? Oikos, 119(7), 1192–1200. https ://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0706.2009.18026.x

  • 1166  |    Journal of Animal Ecology SMITH eT al.

    Amaya-Márgez, M., & Wells, H. (2008). Social complexity and learning foraging tasks in bees. Caldasia, 30(2), 469–477.

    Anderson, E., & Hubricht, L. (1940). A method for describing and com-paring blooming-seasons. Bulletin of the Torrey Botanical Club, 67(8), 639–648. https ://doi.org/10.2307/2481328

    Barrett, S. C. H., & Harder, L. D. (1996). Ecology and evolution of plant mating. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 11(2), 73–79. https ://doi.org/10.1016/0169-5347(96)81046-9

    Bartomeus, I., Ascher, J. S., Gibbs, J., Danforth, B. N., Wagner, D. L., Hedtke, S. M., & Winfree, R. (2013). Historical changes in northeast-ern US bee pollinators related to shared ecological traits. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(12), 4656–4660. https ://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.12185 03110

    Bartomeus, I., Cariveau, D. P., Harrison, T., & Winfree, R. (2018). On the inconsistency of pollinator species traits for predicting either re-sponse to land-use change or functional contribution. Oikos, 127(2), 306–315. https ://doi.org/10.1111/oik.04507

    Bartón, K. (2017). MuMIN: multi-model inference. R package version 1.40.0. Retrieved from https ://cran.r-proje ct.org/packa ge=MuMIn

    Bascompte, J., Jordano, P., & Olesen, J. M. (2006). Asymmetric coevo-lutionary networks facilitate biodiversity maintenance. Science, 312, 431–433. https ://doi.org/10.1126/scien ce.1123412

    Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. https ://doi.org/10.18637/ jss.v067.i01

    Brosi, B. J. (2016). Pollinator specialization: From the individual to the community. New Phytologist, 210(4), 1190–1194. https ://doi.org/10.1111/nph.13951

    Brown, J. H., Gillooly, J. F., Allen, A. P., Savage, V. M., & West, G. B. (2004). Toward a metabolic theory of ecology. Ecology, 85(7), 1771–1789. https ://doi.org/10.1890/03-9000

    Byrne, R. W., & Bates, L. A. (2007). Sociality, evolution and cogni-tion. Current Biology, 17(16), 714–723. https ://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.05.069

    Campbell, D. R., & Motten, A. F. (1985). The mechanism of competition for pollination between two forest herbs. Ecology, 66(2), 554–563. https ://doi.org/10.2307/1940404

    Cane, J. (1987). Estimation of bee size using intertegular span (Apoidea). Journal of the Kansas Entomological Society, 60(1), 145–147. https ://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0050

    Cariveau, D. P., Nayak, G. K., Bartomeus, I., Zientek, J., Ascher, J. S., Gibbs, J., & Winfree, R. (2016). The allometry of bee proboscis length and its uses in ecology. PLoS ONE, 11(3), 1–13. https ://doi.org/10.1371/journ al.pone.0151482

    Castilla, A. R., Pope, N. S., O’Connell, M., Rodriguez, M. F., Treviño, L., Santos, A., & Jha, S. (2017). Adding landscape genetics and individual traits to the ecosystem function paradigm reveals the importance of species functional breadth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(48), 12761–12766. https ://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.16192 71114

    Chittka, L., Thomson, J. D., & Waser, N. (1999). Flower constancy, insect psychology, and plant evolution. Naturwissenschaften, 86, 361–377. https ://doi.org/10.1007/s0011 40050636

    Craine, J. M., Wolkovich, E. M., Towne, E. G., & Kembel, S. W. (2012). Flowering phenology as a functional trait in a tallgrass prairie. New Phytologist, 193, 673–682. https ://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8137.2011.03953.x

    Cripps, C., & Rust, R. W. (1989). Pollen foraging in a community of Osmia bees (Hymenoptera: Megachilidae). Environmental Entomology, 18(4), 582–589. https ://doi.org/10.1093/ee/18.4.582

    Danforth, B. N., Cardinal, S., Praz, C., Almeida, E. A. B., & Michez, D. (2013). The impact of molecular data on our understanding of bee phylogeny and evolution. Annual Review of Entomology, 58(1), 57–78. https ://doi.org/10.1146/annur ev-ento-120811-153633

    Dukas, R., & Real, L. A. (1991). Learning foraging tasks by bees: A com-parison between social and solitary species. Animal Behaviour, 42, 269–276. https ://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80558-5

    Eckhardt, M., Haider, M., Dorn, S., & Müller, A. (2014). Pollen mixing in pollen generalist solitary bees: A possible strategy to complement or mitigate unfavourable pollen properties? Journal of Animal Ecology, 83(3), 588–597. https ://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2656.12168

    Flanagan, R. J., Mitchell, R. J., & Karron, J. D. (2011). Effects of multi-ple competitors for pollination on bumblebee foraging patterns and Mimulus ringens reproductive success. Oikos, 120(2), 200–207. https ://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0706.2010.18777.x

    Flanagan, R. J., Mitchell, R. J., Knutowski, D., & Karron, J. D. (2009). Interspecific pollinator movements reduce pollen deposition and seed production in Mimulus ringens (Phrymaceae). American Journal of Botany, 96(4), 809–815. https ://doi.org/10.3732/ajb.0800317

    Free, J. B. (1963). The flower constancy of honeybees. Journal of Animal Ecology, 32(1), 119–131. https ://doi.org/10.2307/2521

    Gegear, R. J., & Laverty, T. M. (2005). Flower constancy in bumblebees: A test of the trait variability hypothesis. Animal Behaviour, 69(4), 939–949. https ://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbeh av.2004.06.029

    Gelman, A., & Hill, J. (2007). Data analysis using regression and multilevel/hierarchical models. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Gibbs, J., Ascher, J. S., Rightmyer, M. G., & Isaacs, R. (2017). The bees of Michigan (Hymenoptera: Apoidea: Anthophila), with notes on distri-bution, taxonomy, pollination, and natural history. Zootaxa, 4352(1), 1. https ://doi.org/10.11646/ zoota xa.4352.1.1

    Grant, V. (1950). The flower constancy of bees. The Botanical Review, 16(7), 379–398. https ://doi.org/10.1007/BF028 69992

    Greenleaf, S. S., Williams, N. M., Winfree, R., & Kremen, C. (2007). Bee foraging ranges and their relationship to body size. Oecologia, 153, 589–596. https ://doi.org/10.1007/s00442-007-0752-9

    Harrison, T., Gibbs, J., & Winfree, R. (2018). Forest bees are replaced in agricultural and urban landscapes by native species with differ-ent phenologies and life-history traits. Global Change Biology, 24(1), 287–296. https ://doi.org/10.1111/gcb.13921

    Heinrich, B. (1976). Flowering phenologies: Bog, woodland, and disturbed habitats. Ecology, 57(5), 890–899. https ://doi.org/10.2307/1941055

    Hicks, D. M., Ouvrard, P., Baldock, K. C. R., Baude, M., Goddard, M. A., Kunin, W. E., … Stone, G. N. (2016). Food for pollinators: Quantifying the nectar and pollen resources of urban flower meadows. PLoS ONE, 11(6), 1–37. https ://doi.org/10.1371/journ al.pone.0158117

    Johnson, P. C. D. (2014). Extension of Nakagawa & Schielzeth’s R2GLMM to random slopes models. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 5, 944–946. https ://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.12225

    Jones, P. L., & Agrawal, A. A. (2017). Learning in insect pollinators and herbivores. Annual Review of Entomology, 62(1), 53–71. https ://doi.org/10.1146/annur ev-ento-031616-034903

    Jost, L. (2006). Entropy and diversity. Oikos, 113, 363–375. https ://doi.org/10.1111/j.2006.0030-1299.14714.x

    Kaiser-Bunbury, C. N., Mougal, J., Whittington, A. E., Valentin, T., Gabriel, R., Olesen, J. M., & Blüthgen, N. (2017). Ecosystem restoration strengthens pollination network resilience and function. Nature, 542(7640), 223–227. https ://doi.org/10.1038/natur e21071

    Kearns, C. A., & Inouye, D. W. (1993). Techniques for pollination biologists (1st ed.). Niwot, CO: University Press of Colorado.

    Keller, A., Danner, N., Grimmer, G., Ankenbrand, M., von der Ohe, K., von der Ohe, W., … Steffan-Dewenter, I. (2015). Evaluating multiplexed next-generation sequencing as a method in palynology for mixed pollen samples. Plant Biology, 17, 558–566. https ://doi.org/10.1111/plb.12251

    Kochmer, J. P., & Handel, S. N. (1986). Constraints and competition in the evolution of flowering phenology. Ecology, 56(4), 303–325.

    Kunin, W. E. (1993). Sex and the single mustard: Population density and pollinator behavior effects on seed set. Ecology, 74(7), 2145–2160. https ://doi.org/10.2307/1940859

    LaBerge, W. E. (1986). A revision of the bees of the genus Andrena of the Western Hemisphere. Part XI. Minor subgenera and subgeneric key. Transactions of the American Entomological Society, 111, 440–567.

  •      |  1167Journal of Animal EcologySMITH eT al.

    LaBerge, W. E. (1987). A revision of the bees of the genus Andrena of the Western Hemisphere. Part XII. Subgenera Leucandrena, Ptilandrena, Scoliandrena, and Melandrena. Transactions of the American Entomological Society, 112, 191–248.

    Larsen, T. H., Williams, N. M., & Kremen, C. (2005). Extinction order and altered community structure rapidly disrupt eco-system functioning. Ecology Letters, 8(5), 538–547. https ://doi.org/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2005.00749.x

    Leonhardt, S. D., & Blüthgen, N. (2012). The same, but different: Pollen foraging in honeybee and bumblebee colonies. Apidologie, 43(4), 449–464. https ://doi.org/10.1007/s13592-011-0112-y

    Lopezaraiza-Mikel, M. E., Hayes, R. B., Whalley, M. R., & Memmott, J. (2007). The impact of an alien plant on a native plant-pollinator net-work: An experimental approach. Ecology Letters, 10(7), 539–550. https ://doi.org/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2007.01055.x

    Lucas, A., Bodger, O., Brosi, B. J., Ford, C. R., Forman, D. W., Greig, C., … de Vere, N. (2018). Generalisation and specialisation in hoverfly (Syrphidae) grassland pollen transport networks revealed by DNA metabarcoding. Journal of Animal Ecology, 87(4), 1008–1021. https ://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2656.12828

    Makrodimos, N., Blionis, G. J., Krigas, N., & Vokou, D. (2008). Flower morphology, phenology and visitor patterns in an alpine commu-nity on Mt Olympos, Greece. Flora, 203, 449–468. https ://doi.org/10.1016/j.flora.2007.07.003

    Michener, C. D. (2000). Bees of the world. Baltimore, MD: The Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Michener, C. D., & Rettenmeyer, C. W. (1956). The ethology of Andrena erythronii with comparative data on other species (Hymenoptera, Andrenidae). The University of Kansas Science Bulletin, 37(16), 645–684.

    Minckley, R. L., & Roulston, T. H. (2006). Incidental mutualisms and pol-len specialization among bees. In N. Waser, & J. Ollerton (Eds.), Plant-pollinator interactions: From specialization to generalization (pp. 69–98). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Morales, C., & Traveset, A. (2008). Interspecific pollen transfer: Magnitude, prevalence and consequences for plant fitness. Critical Reviews in Plant Sciences, 27(4), 221–238. https ://doi.org/10.1080/07352 68080 2205631

    Morales, M. A., Dodge, G. J., & Inouye, D. W. (2005). A phenological mid-domain effect in flowering diversity. Oecologia, 142(1), 83–89. https ://doi.org/10.1007/s00442-004-1694-0

    Motten, A. F. (1986). Pollination ecology of the spring wildflower com-munity of a temperate deciduous forest. Ecological Monographs, 56(1), 21–42. https ://doi.org/10.2307/2937269

    Müller, A. (1996). Host-plant specialization in western Palearctic Anthidine bees (Hymenoptera: Apoidea: Megachilidae). Ecological Monographs, 66(2), 235–257. https ://doi.org/10.2307/2963476

    Müller, A., Diener, S., Schnyder, S., Stutz, K., Sedivy, C., & Dorn, S. (2006). Quantitative pollen requirements of solitary bees: Implications for bee conservation and the evolution of bee-flower relationships. Biological Conservation, 130(4), 604–615. https ://doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2006.01.023

    Nakagawa, S., & Schielzeth, H. (2013). A general and simple method for obtaining R2 from generalized linear mixed-effects mod-els. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 4, 133–142. https ://doi.org/10.1111/j.2041-210x.2012.00261.x

    Ogilvie, J. E., & Forrest, J. R. (2017). Interactions between bee forag-ing and floral resource phenology shape bee populations and com-munities. Current Opinion in Insect Science, 21, 75–82. https ://doi.org/10.1016/j.cois.2017.05.015

    Pornon, A., Andalo, C., Burrus, M., & Escaravage, N. (2017). DNA me-tabarcoding data unveils invisible pollination networks. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 1–11. https ://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-16785-5

    Praz, C. J., Müller, A., & Dorn, S. (2008). Specialized bees fail to de-velop on non-host pollen: Do plants chemically protect their pollen? Ecology, 89(3), 795–804. https ://doi.org/10.1890/07-0751.1

    R Core Team. (2018). R: A language and environment for statistical com-puting. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from https ://www.r-proje ct.org/

    Ramalho, M., Giannini, T. C., Malagodi-Braga, K. S., & Imperatriz-Fonseca, V. L. (1994). Pollen harvest by stingless bee foragers (Hymenoptera, Apidae, Meliponinae). Grana, 33(4–5), 239–244. https ://doi.org/10.1080/00173 13940 9429005

    Reiss, M. (1988). Scaling of home range size: Body size, metabolic needs and ecology. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 3(3), 85–86. https ://doi.org/10.1016/0169-5347(88)90025-0

    Schielzeth, H. (2010). Simple means to improve the interpretability of regression coefficients. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 1(2), 103–113. https ://doi.org/10.1111/j.2041-210X.2010.00012.x

    Smith, C., Weinman, L. R., Gibbs, J., & Winfree, R. (2019). Data from: Specialist foragers in forest bee communities are small, social or emerge early. Dryad Digital Repository, https ://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.2bm34c2

    Tur, C., Vigalondo, B., Trøjelsgaard, K., Olesen, J. M., & Traveset, A. (2014). Downscaling pollen-transport networks to the level of in-dividuals. Journal of Animal Ecology, 83(1), 306–317. https ://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2656.12130

    USDA & NRCS. (2018). The PLANTS database. Greensboro, NC: National Plant Data Team. http://plants.usda.gov

    Vamosi, J. C., Knight, T. M., Steets, J. A., Mazer, S. J., Burd, M., & Ashman, T.-L. (2006). Pollination decays in biodiversity hotspots. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(4), 956–961. https ://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.05071 65103

    Waser, N., Chittka, L., Price, M. V., Williams, N. M., & Ollerton, J. (1996). Generalization in pollination systems, and why it matters. Ecology, 77(4), 1043–1060. https ://doi.org/10.2307/2265575

    Williams, N. M. (2003). Use of novel pollen species by specialist and gen-eralist solitary bees (Hymenoptera: Megachilidae). Oecologia, 134, 228–237. https ://doi.org/10.1007/s00442-002-1104-4

    Wood, T. J., Gibbs, J., Rothwell, N., Wilson, J. K., Gut, L., Brokaw, J., & Isaacs, R. (2018). Limited phenological and dietary overlap between bee communities in spring flowering crops and herbaceous en-hancements. Ecological Applications, 28(7), 1924–1934. https ://doi.org/10.1002/eap.1789

    Zuur, A. F., Ieno, E. N., Walker, N. J., Saveliev, A. A., & Smith, G. M. (2009). Mixed effects models and extensions in ecology with R (1st ed.). New York, NY: Springer.

    SUPPORTING INFORMATION

    Additional supporting information may be found online in the Supporting Information section at the end of the article.

    How to cite this article: Smith C, Weinman L, Gibbs J, Winfree R. Specialist foragers in forest bee communities are small, social or emerge early. J Anim Ecol. 2019;88:1158–1167. https ://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2656.13003

  • 1

    Appendix S1: Supplementary methods, results, tables and figures 1

    2

    Supplementary methods and results 3

    Pollen load diversity measurements using Hill numbers 4

    We used a Hill numbers approach to measuring diversity because this is theoretically 5

    preferable to other diversity metrics and provides a way to explicitly indicate the emphasis given 6

    to rare versus common species (Jost, 2006). Hill diversities are based on one general equation, of 7

    which species richness and forms of the traditional Shannon and Simpson indices are special 8

    cases (Jost, 2006). Specifically, Hill-Shannon is the traditional Shannon index exponentiated, 9

    and Hill-Simpson is the inverse of the traditional Simpson index. All Hill diversities are in the 10

    same units, called the effective number of species, which is the number of species that would be 11

    present in a perfectly even community (i.e., one in which each species was represented by the 12

    same number of individuals) with the same diversity metric value as the one being studied. 13

    We calculated pollen load diversity with Hill numbers using incidence data (that is, the 14

    frequencies with which pollen morphotypes occur across multiple microscope scans of a slide). 15

    For the ith pollen morphotype in a pollen load, !! is the proportion of scans containing that 16

    morphotype, and !! = !!!!!! , the relative incidence of the pollen morphotype in the pollen load. 17

    Hill numbers based off incidence data are described by the following equation (Chao et al., 18

    2014): 19

    ! ! = p!!

    !

    !!!

    !!!!

    for q ≥ 0 q ≠ 1

    exp − p! lnp!!

    !!! for q = 1

  • 2

    where S = the number of pollen morphotypes in the individual pollen load and q = the diversity 20

    order, which is used to indicate the Hill number’s sensitivity to rare or common species. When q 21

    = 0, ! ! is species richness, which is sensitive to rare species; when q =1, ! ! is Hill-Shannon 22

    diversity, which provides a balanced measure of rare and common species; and when q=2, ! ! is 23

    Hill-Simpson diversity, which is more sensitive to common species. 24

    We use a diversity order of q = 0 (i.e., species richness) for our main analysis. To ensure 25

    our results were robust to our choice of diversity metric, we also reran our analyses using 26

    diversity orders of q = 1 (i.e., Hill-Shannon diversity) and q = 2 (i.e., Hill-Simpson diversity). 27

    Our results were qualitatively the same between the three different diversity orders (Fig. 28

    S2). In the analyses of all three diversity orders, there were significant main effects of site, day-29

    of-year, sociality and body size, and significant interactions between day-of-year and lecty and 30

    between day-of-year and plant growth form (Fig. S2). 31

    32

    Comparing the distributions of individual pollen load diversities across bee species 33

    We examined the distributions of individual pollen load diversities for nine common bee 34

    species in our data (N ≥ 20). All species we examined had right skewed pollen load diversity 35

    distributions, with most individuals carrying low diversity pollen loads (Fig. S3). However, the 36

    percentage of individuals carrying pure pollen loads varied widely among species, ranging from 37

    85.2% of Andrena zizeae to only 4.0% of A. nasonii (Table S6). The median richness of 38

    individual pollen loads ranged from one (in A. zizeae, C. inaequalis, and L. subvirdatum) to three 39

    (in B. bimaculatus; Table S6). 40

    41

    42

  • 3

    References 43

    Chao, A., Gotelli, N. J., Hsieh, T. C., Sander, E. L., Ma, K. H., Colwell, R. K., & Ellison, A. M. 44

    (2014). Rarefaction and extrapolation with Hill numbers: a framework for sampling and 45

    estimation in species diversity studies. Ecological Monographs, 84(1), 45–67. 46

    doi:10.1890/13-0133.1 47

    Jost, L. (2006). Entropy and diversity. Oikos, 113, 363–375. doi:10.1111/j.2006.0030-48

    1299.14714.x 49

    50

  • 4

    Supplementary tables 51 52 Table S1. Locations of study sites. 53 54 Site Name Location Latitude Longitude Hutcheson Memorial Forest USA, NJ, Somerset County 40.5006 -74.5639 Institute Woods USA, NJ, Mercer County 40.3240 -74.6631 Sourland Mountain Preserve USA, NJ, Somerset County 40.4731 -74.6956 Bowman's Hill USA, PA, Bucks County 40.3285 -74.9432

    55 56

  • 5

    Table S2. Plant species and the number of bees collected off of each. 57

    Plant species Bees collected Acer negundo 12 Acer platanoides 1 Acer rubrum 38 Anemone canadensis 3 Aquilegia canadensis 5 Aronia arbutifolia 2 Barbarea vulgaris 54 Cardamine impatiens 3 Cerastium nutans 1 Cerastium spp. 1 Cercis canadensis 4 Chrysogonum virginianum 2 Cornus alternifolia 8 Cornus florida 3 Dicentra cucullaria 8 Dodecatheon meadia 2 Eleagnus umbellata 6 Erythronium americanum 5 Euphorbia esula 9 Geranium maculatum 11 Glechoma hederacea 10 Hepatica nobilis_var._obtusa 1 Hydrophyllum virginianum 11 Lamium purpureum 2 Lonicera morrowii 4 Malus pumila 7 Malus spp. 19 Mertensia virginica 38 Packera aurea 8 Physocarpus opulifolius 3 Polemonium reptans 2 Prunus americana 7 Prunus avium 45 Pyrus calleryana 9 Ranunculus bulbosus 11 Ranunculus ficaria 8 Rhododendron calendulaceum 1 Rhododendron periclymenoides 2 Rhododendron prinophyllum 2 Rubus pensilvanicus 2

  • 6

    Stylophorum diphyllum 6 Taraxacum officinale 29 Tradescantia virginiana 2 Trifolium repens 2 Viburnum opulus 1 Viola spp. 2 Zizia aurea 30

    58

    59

  • 7

    Table S3. Pollen morphotypes (n=46) found on the bees in our study. Thirty pollen morphotypes 60

    were resolved to the species level. The rest are grouped by clade, family or genus. For each 61

    pollen morphotype, we list the taxonomic level of the grouping and the mean relative incidence 62

    of the morphotype across all pollen loads. The five most common pollen morphotypes were Acer 63

    (17.9%), Rosaceae (16.4%), Brassicaceae (12.0%), Mertensia virginica (10.5%) and Zizea aurea 64

    (6.7%). 65

    66

    Pollen group Taxonomic level of grouping Mean (%) SE (%) Alliaria petiolata / Euphorbia esula Clade 0.99 0.27 Euonymus alata / Viburnum prunifolium Clade 0.59 0.19 Anemone / Aquilegia Family 1.64 0.54 Asteraceae Family 1.60 0.49 Betulaceae Family 0.32 0.24 Brassicaceae Family 12.04 1.25 Duchesnea indica/ Fragaria virginiana Family 0.46 0.19 Lauraceae Family 0.53 0.25 Ranunculaceae Family 3.02 0.68 Rosaceae Family 16.42 1.38 Acer Genus 17.92 1.52 Carex Genus 0.07 0.07 Cerastium Genus 0.26 0.22 Lonicera Genus 0.61 0.28 Magnolia Genus 0.07 0.05 Rhododendron Genus 1.20 0.48 Viola Genus 0.27 0.19 Ajuga reptans Species 0.17 0.12 Cercis canadensis Species 1.49 0.45 Claytonia virginica Species 0.72 0.29 Cornus alternifolia Species 1.60 0.54 Cornus florida Species 0.92 0.25 Dicentra cucullaria Species 0.55 0.22 Dodecatheon meadia Species 0.50 0.30 Duchesnea indica Species 0.08 0.09 Eleagnus umbellata Species 0.61 0.25 Erythronium americanum Species 0.50 0.25 Geranium maculatum Species 0.25 0.22 Glechoma hederacea Species 0.35 0.17

  • 8

    Hydrophyllum virginianum Species 1.33 0.44 Lamiaceae Species 0.01 0.01 Lamium purpureum Species 0.09 0.07 Liriodendron tulipifera Species 1.11 0.31 Mertensia virginica Species 10.49 1.33 Physocarpus opulifolius Species 0.08 0.08 Polemonium reptans Species 0.40 0.24 Rubus pensilvanicus Species 0.06 0.06 Sassafras albidum Species 0.04 0.03 Saxifraga pensylvanica Species 0.02 0.02 Saxifraga virginensis Species 0.11 0.11 Stylophorum diphyllum Species 0.78 0.28 Taraxacum officinale Species 5.41 0.89 Tradescantia virginiana Species 0.48 0.20 Trifolium repens Species 0.30 0.15 Viburnum opulus Species 0.06 0.07 Zizia aurea Species 6.74 1.16 Various morphospecies NA 6.72 0.75

    67 68 69

  • 9

    Table S4. Species’ traits used in the analysis, the number of individual bees and bee species in 70

    each trait category, as well as the number of plant species that individual bees in each trait 71

    category were collected from. Two individuals from two facultatively social bee species were 72

    categorized as social. 73

    74 75

    Trait Values Individual bees Bee species Plant species

    Sociality Social 104 21 33

    Solitary 339 35 42

    Lecty Oligolectic 47 7 14

    Polylectic 396 49 47

    Growth form Herbaceous 266 50 28

    Woody 177 30 21

    Body size Range: 1.2 to 121.5 mg 443 56 49 76

  • 10

    Table S5. List of bee species we removed pollen from and the number of individuals of each 77

    species collected. 78

    79 Species Authority n Andrena alleghaniensis Viereck, 1907 1 Andrena banksi Malloch, 1917 3 Andrena carlini Cockerell, 1901 26 Andrena cornelli Viereck, 1907 4 Andrena cressonii Robertson, 1891 12 Andrena distans Provancher, 1888 1 Andrena erigeniae Robertson, 1891 3 Andrena erythronii Robertson, 1891 7 Andrena fenningeri Viereck, 1922 6 Andrena forbesii Robertson, 1891 6 Andrena heraclei Robertson, 1897 2 Andrena hippotes Robertson, 1895 3 Andrena imitatrix Cresson, 1872 24 Andrena integra Smith, 1853 2 Andrena mandibularis Robertson, 1892 13 Andrena miserabilis Cresson, 1872 12 Andrena nasonii Robertson, 1895 24 Andrena perplexa Smith, 1853 1 Andrena pruni Robertson, 1891 5 Andrena robertsonii Dalla Torre, 1896 2 Andrena rugosa Cockerell, 1906 14 Andrena vicina Smith, 1853 4 Andrena ziziae Robertson, 1891 27 Augochlora pura (Say, 1837) 16 Augochloropsis metallica (Fabricius, 1793) 4 Bombus bimaculatus Cresson, 1863 24 Bombus griseocollis (De Geer, 1773) 6 Bombus impatiens Cresson, 1863 1 Bombus perplexus Cresson, 1863 5 Colletes inaequalis Say, 1837 24 Lasioglossum abanci (Crawford, 1932) 2 Lasioglossum birkmanni (Crawford, 1906) 2 Lasioglossum cattellae (Ellis, 1913) 1 Lasioglossum coeruleum (Robertson, 1893) 5 Lasioglossum coriaceum (Smith, 1853) 1 Lasioglossum cressonii (Robertson, 1890) 9 Lasioglossum foxii (Robertson, 1895) 19

  • 11

    Lasioglossum fuscipenne (Smith, 1853) 1 Lasioglossum gotham Gibbs, 2011 2 Lasioglossum illinoense (Robertson, 1892) 1 Lasioglossum imitatum (Smith, 1853) 6 Lasioglossum obscurum (Robertson, 1892) 1 Lasioglossum pilosum (Smith, 1853) 1 Lasioglossum quebecense (Crawford, 1907) 6 Lasioglossum subviridatum (Cockerell, 1938) 21 Lasioglossum truncatum (Robertson, 1901) 6 Lasioglossum versatum (Robertson, 1902) 5 Lasioglossum viridatum (Lovell, 1905) 1 Lasioglossum weemsi (Mitchell, 1960) 1 Osmia atriventris Cresson, 1864 15 Osmia bucephala Cresson, 1864 1 Osmia collinsiae Robertson, 1905 2 Osmia conjuncta Cresson, 1864 1 Osmia georgica Cresson, 1878 3 Osmia lignaria Say, 1837 1 Osmia pumila Cresson, 1864 47

    80 81

  • 12

    Table S6. The nine most common bee species in our dataset (N ≥ 20), with the number of 82

    individuals collected, the percentage of individuals carrying pure (i.e. single morphotype) pollen 83

    loads, and the median richness of pollen loads. 84

    Bee species N % pure pollen loads Median richness Osmia pumila 47 17.0 2.0 Andrena ziziae 27 85.2 1.0 Andrena carlini 26 7.7 2.5 Andrena imitatrix 24 45.8 2.0 Andrena nasonii 24 4.2 2.0 Bombus bimaculatus 24 25.0 3.0 Colletes inaequalis 24 58.3 1.0 Lasioglossum subviridatum 21 57.1 1.0

    85

  • 13

    Table S7. Results of model selection: AICc values, ΔAICc values and weights of models within 86

    ten AICc values of the best-fitting model. All models included day-of-year as a main fixed effect 87

    and bee species as a random intercept and slope effect. 88

    day site growth lecty size sociality day: site day: growth day: lecty day: size day: sociality AICc ΔAICc wi + + + + + +

    + + +

    606.53 0.00 0.251

    + + + + + +

    + +

    607.78 1.25 0.134 + + + + + +

    + + + + 608.69 2.16 0.085

    + +

    + + +

    + +

    609.32 2.79 0.062 + + + + + +

    + +

    + 609.93 3.40 0.046

    + +

    + + +

    + + + 610.44 3.90 0.036 + + + + + +

    + +

    610.47 3.94 0.035

    + +

    + + +

    +

    611.22 4.69 0.024 + + +

    + +

    +

    +

    611.43 4.90 0.022

    + + + + + +

    + + + 611.93 5.40 0.017 + + + + + +

    +

    +

    611.94 5.41 0.017

    + + + + + +

    +

    612.25 5.71 0.014 + +

    + + + +

    + +

    612.28 5.75 0.014

    + +

    + +

    + +

    612.30 5.77 0.014 + +

    + + +

    +

    + 612.40 5.87 0.013

    +

    + + +

    + +

    612.46 5.93 0.013 + + + + +

    + + +

    612.62 6.09 0.012

    + + + + + + + + + +

    612.63 6.10 0.012 + + +

    + +

    +

    + + 613.05 6.52 0.010

    + + + + + + + + +

    613.64 7.11 0.007 + + + + + +

    +

    + + 613.70 7.17 0.007

    +

    + + + +

    + +

    613.71 7.18 0.007 + + + + + +

    +

    + 613.72 7.19 0.007

    +

    + + +

    +

    613.86 7.32 0.006 + +

    + + + +

    +

    613.91 7.38 0.006

    +

    + + +

    + + + 613.99 7.45 0.006 + + +

    + +

    +

    614.13 7.60 0.006

    +

    + + + +

    + + +

    614.20 7.67 0.005 + + + + +

    + +

    614.23 7.70 0.005

    + + + + + + +

    + +

    614.38 7.85 0.005 + +

    + + + +

    + + + 614.38 7.85 0.005

    + + + + + + + + + + + 614.81 8.27 0.004 + + + + +

    + +

    614.83 8.30 0.004

    +

    + + + +

    + +

    614.84 8.31 0.004 + + + + + +

    +

    614.89 8.36 0.004

    + +

    + +

    +

    615.00 8.47 0.004 +

    + + + +

    +

    615.01 8.48 0.004

    +

    + + +

    +

    + 615.31 8.78 0.003 + + +

    +

    +

    +

    615.42 8.89 0.003

    +

    + + + +

    + + + 615.50 8.96 0.003

  • 14

    + +

    + +

    +

    615.55 9.02 0.003 + + +

    + +

    +

    + 615.65 9.12 0.003

    + + + + + + + + +

    + 615.81 9.28 0.002 + + + + + + +

    +

    615.85 9.31 0.002

    + +

    + + + +

    +

    + 615.87 9.33 0.002 + + +

    + +

    +

    615.94 9.41 0.002

    + +

    + + +

    +

    615.96 9.43 0.002 + +

    +

    +

    616.19 9.65 0.002

    +

    + + + +

    + + + + 616.32 9.79 0.002 + +

    + + +

    +

    616.38 9.85 0.002

    + + + + + + + + + + 616.42 9.89 0.002 89

    90

  • 15

    Table S8. Oligolectic bee species collected, their host plants, and the pollen found in their pollen 91

    loads. Pollen taxa from a bee species’ host plant is highlighted in gray. 92

    Bee species N Host plant Pollen Mean (%) S.E. Andrena cornelli 4 Rhododendron spp. Rhododendron spp. 100.00 0.00 Andrena distans 1 Geranium maculatum Geranium maculatum 100.00 NA Andrena erigeniae 4 Claytonia virginica Claytonia virginica 75.76 15.30

    Ranunculus spp. 24.24 15.30

    Andrena erythronii 7 Erythronium spp. Acer spp. 31.19 9.16

    Taraxacum officinale 25.78 7.54

    Rosaceae 16.68 7.98

    Stylophorum diphyllum 10.95 7.11

    Erythronium americanum 8.57 7.05

    Claytonia virginica 4.63 4.15

    Betulaceae 1.05 1.05

    Morphospecies1 0.71 0.71

    Brassicaceae 0.43 0.43

    Andrena integra 2 Cornus (Swida) spp. Cornus alternifolia 95.45 4.55

    Zizia aurea 4.55 4.55

    Andrena ziziae 27 Zizia spp. Zizia aurea 98.35 0.82

    Morphospecies2 1.45 0.81

    Rosaceae 0.21 0.21

    Osmia georgica 3 Asteraceae Asteraceae 81.79 11.65

    Ranunculus 16.76 12.31

    Zizia aurea 1.45 1.45 93

    94

    95

  • 16

    Supplementary Figures 96

    97

    98

    Figure S1. Increase in floral diversity over time, with floral diversity measured as the rarefied 99

    richness of plant species bees were collected from. To estimate floral diversity, we used all of 100

    our data (N= 1301 bees) including males, parasitic species, and non-forest bees. We used 101

    rarefaction to estimate plant species richness at an equal sample size (n = 26) for each site-date. 102

    We excluded 10 site-dates which did not meet a minimum sample size of 25 bees. 103

    ●●

    ●●

    Apr 01 Apr 15 May 01 May 15

    24

    68

    Date

    Flor

    al d

    ivers

    ity (R

    aref

    ied

    richn

    ess)

  • 17

    104

    Figure S2. Results of parametric boostrapping analyses when Richness, Hill-Shannon diversity 105

    and Hill-Simpson diversity (all loge-transformed) are used to measure pollen load diversity. The 106

    points represent parameter estimates and lines represent bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals. 107

    Points are in red if the bootstrapped confidence intervals do not overlap with zero. 108

    109

    Hill−Shannon Hill−Simpson Richness

    −0.5 0.0 0.5 −0.5 0.0 0.5 −0.5 0.0 0.5

    Day−of−year

    Day−of−year:Growth

    Day−of−year:Lecty

    Day−of−year:Size

    Growth

    Intercept

    Lecty

    Site2

    Site3

    Site4

    Size

    Sociality

    Estimate

    Parameter Significant

    NoYes

  • 18

    110

    Figure S3. The distributions of individual pollen load diversities of common species (N ≥ 20), 111

    with pollen load diversity measured using species richness. 112

    113

    114 115

    Osmia pumila N= 47

    0 2 4 6 8

    0.0

    0.2

    0.4

    0.6

    0.8

    Andrena ziziae N= 27

    0 2 4 6 8

    0.0

    0.2

    0.4

    0.6

    0.8

    Andrena carlini N= 26

    0 2 4 6 8

    0.0

    0.2

    0.4

    0.6

    0.8

    Andrena imitatrix N= 24

    0 2 4 6 8

    0.0

    0.2

    0.4

    0.6

    0.8

    Andrena nasonii N= 24

    0 2 4 6 8

    0.0

    0.2

    0.4

    0.6

    0.8

    Bombus bimaculatus N= 24

    0 2 4 6 80.

    00.

    20.

    40.

    60.

    8Colletes inaequalis

    N= 24

    0 2 4 6 8

    0.0

    0.2

    0.4

    0.6

    0.8

    Lasioglossum subviridatum N= 21

    0 2 4 6 8

    0.0

    0.2

    0.4

    0.6

    0.8

    Pollen load diversity (Richness)

    Freq

    uenc

    y

  • 19

    116 Figure S4. Results of parametric boostrapping analyses of the top four ranked models (all ΔAICc 117

    < 3) explaining pollen load richness (loge-transformed). The points represent parameter estimates 118

    and lines represent bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals. Points are in red if the bootstrapped 119

    confidence intervals do not overlap with zero. Results were qualitatively similar between the top 120

    four ranked models, with one exception. The fourth-ranked model did not include the variable 121

    plant growth form, or its interaction with day-of-year; this interaction was significant in the three 122

    other top-ranked models. The fourth-ranked model instead included a significant interaction 123

    Rank 3 Rank 4

    Rank 1 Rank 2

    −0.5 0.0 0.5 −0.5 0.0 0.5

    Day−of−yearDay−of−year:Growth

    Day−of−year:LectyDay−of−year:Size

    Day−of−year:SocialGrowth

    InterceptLectySite2Site3Site4Size

    Sociality

    Day−of−yearDay−of−year:Growth

    Day−of−year:LectyDay−of−year:Size

    Day−of−year:SocialGrowth

    InterceptLectySite2Site3Site4Size

    Sociality

    Estimate

    Parameter Significant

    NoYes

  • 20

    between day-of-year and body size, with day-of-year having a greater effect on the diversity of 124

    pollen carried by large bees than on the diversity of pollen carried by small bees. 125

    126

    127

  • 21

    128

    Figure S5. Box plots illustrating the difference in the richness of pollen carried by individuals of 129

    solitary (N=27) and social bee (N=55) species in the genus Lasioglossum. To minimize co-130

    linearity between body size and sociality, we excluded the eight smallest individuals of social 131

    Lasioglossum, which were outliers in terms of their body size [log10(body size) < 0.40]. The 132

    boxes encompass the second and third quartiles of the data and the thick black line is the median. 133

    The plot whiskers extend to 1.5 times the interquartile range and circles represent outliers. 134

    ●●

    Solitary Social

    12

    34

    56

    7

    Pollen load diversities of social and solitary Lasioglossum

    Sociality

    Polle

    n lo

    ad ri

    chne

    ss


Recommended