+ All Categories
Home > Documents > XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

Date post: 13-May-2017
Category:
Upload: gempok
View: 226 times
Download: 2 times
Share this document with a friend
39
Electronic copy available at: http://ssrn.com/abstract=1869013 i XBRL-Enabled, Excel or PDF? The Effects of Exclusive Technology Choice on the Analysis of Financial Information Diane Janvrin Associate Professor of Accounting Iowa State University 3365 Gerdin Business Building Ames, IA 50011 Phone: (515) 294-9450 Fax: 515 294 3525 Email: [email protected] Robert Pinsker Assistant Professor of Accounting Florida Atlantic University Boca Raton FL 33431-0991 Phone: (561) 297-3422 Fax: (561) 297-7023 Email: [email protected] Maureen Francis Mascha Assistant Professor of Accounting University of Wisconsin-Oshkosh Email: [email protected] Please do not quote without permission. June 2011 We thank EDGAR-Online for use of their software. We thank Michael Alles, Roger Debreceny, Stephanie Farewell, Carsten Felden, James Hunton, Jon Perkins, Saeed Roohani, Nathan Stuart, and participants at Bryant University, University of Wisconsin – Oshkosh, University of Kansas 2011 XBRL Symposium, 22 nd XBRL International Conference, 2011 South Florida Research Symposium, and Iowa State University Accounting/Finance Research Forum for helpful comments, Sue Childs and Liv Watson for their software assistance and Kevin Den Adel, Jim Kurtenbach, Patrick Wheeler, and Winston Chappell for assisting with the experimental instrument development and data collection.
Transcript
Page 1: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

Electronic copy available at: http://ssrn.com/abstract=1869013

i

XBRL-Enabled, Excel or PDF? The Effects of Exclusive Technology Choice on the Analysis of Financial Information

Diane Janvrin Associate Professor of Accounting

Iowa State University 3365 Gerdin Business Building

Ames, IA 50011 Phone: (515) 294-9450

Fax: 515 294 3525 Email: [email protected]

Robert Pinsker

Assistant Professor of Accounting Florida Atlantic University Boca Raton FL 33431-0991

Phone: (561) 297-3422 Fax: (561) 297-7023

Email: [email protected]

Maureen Francis Mascha Assistant Professor of Accounting University of Wisconsin-Oshkosh Email: [email protected] 

Please do not quote without permission. June 2011

We thank EDGAR-Online for use of their software. We thank Michael Alles, Roger Debreceny, Stephanie Farewell, Carsten Felden, James Hunton, Jon Perkins, Saeed Roohani, Nathan Stuart, and participants at Bryant University, University of Wisconsin – Oshkosh, University of Kansas 2011 XBRL Symposium, 22nd XBRL International Conference, 2011 South Florida Research Symposium, and Iowa State University Accounting/Finance Research Forum for helpful comments, Sue Childs and Liv Watson for their software assistance and Kevin Den Adel, Jim Kurtenbach, Patrick Wheeler, and Winston Chappell for assisting with the experimental instrument development and data collection.

Page 2: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

Electronic copy available at: http://ssrn.com/abstract=1869013

ii

XBRL-Enabled, Excel or PDF? The Effects of Exclusive Technology Choice on the Analysis of Financial Information

Abstract

U.S. adoption of search-facilitating technology has been slow and several constituencies question whether investors will choose to use the XBRL-formatted information the Securities and Exchange Commission (SEC) is now requiring companies to provide. Unlike prior research, we use an exclusive choice experimental design to examine (1) which reporting technology nonprofessional investors will choose to complete a financial analysis task (XBRL-enabled, portable document file, or spreadsheet) and (2) why they choose the specific technology. We found 66 percent of nonprofessional investor proxies chose to use XBRL-enabled technology, while 34 percent chose spreadsheets. Participants who chose XBRL-enabled technology perceived it reduces the time required to complete the task (i.e., increases task efficiency) while participants who chose spreadsheets indicated their technology choice was driven by amount of prior experience with that technology. Our findings have implications for the technology choice literature, regulators mandating or considering mandating XBRL-based reporting, and XBRL-enabled technology adoption. Keywords: exclusive technology choice, user choice, XBRL-enabled technology adoption Data Availability: Contact the authors.

Page 3: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

1

XBRL-Enabled, Excel or PDF? The Effects of Exclusive Technology Choice on the Analysis of Financial Information

I. INTRODUCTION

Technology is a pervasive and growing component of accounting tasks and has been

shown to change work processes (Mauldin and Ruchala 1999, 328). With regard to financial

analysis tasks, recent technology advancements create the opportunity to analyze financial

information more efficiently. For example, eXtensible Business Reporting Language (XBRL)

proponents argue that data reported in this format will be more transparent to nonprofessional

investors (Baldwin et al. 2006; Gunn 2007; XBRL International Standards Board [XSB] 2010).

With individual financial statement items tagged, investors are able to quickly search for and

analyze the information they perceive to be important during the financial analysis process using

search-facilitating (also known as XBRL-enabled) reporting technology; that is parser software

designed to manipulate financial information tagged in XBRL (Hodge et al. 2004; Locke et al.

2009). Thus, XBRL proponents contend that investors who formerly relied on either

spreadsheets (e.g., Excel) or document exchange software (e.g., PDF) to perform financial

analysis tasks may be able to perform these tasks more efficiently with XBRL-enabled reporting

technology.1

Motivated by the goal of assisting nonprofessional investors in understanding financial

information (Cox 2006), the Securities and Exchange Commission (SEC) recently mandated that

all publicly traded firms furnish financial information tagged in XBRL as exhibits to their

quarterly and annual filings (SEC 2009) and place the tagged financial information on their

1 Although prior research (i.e., Hodge et al. 2004; Blankespoor et al. 2011) use the term ‘search-facilitating’ to describe parser software that manipulates financial information tagged in XBRL, we elect to describe this technology as ‘XBRL-enabled’ since investors can also conduct searches using spreadsheets or PDFs. As noted in Section II, searches using spreadsheets or PDFs are often more labor intensive.

Page 4: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

2

Investor Relations web sites. However, despite the significant investment by firms, regulatory

agencies, and academics throughout the world,2 relevant research and anecdotal evidence suggest

that investors may elect to use other reporting technologies rather than XBRL-enabled

technology when performing financial analysis tasks (Hodge et al. 2004). For example, Hodge et

al. (2004) find that despite the purported benefits of XBRL, almost 50 percent of nonprofessional

investor participants did not use XBRL-enabled technology in their information acquisition task.

The extant user choice literature finds that users do not always choose to use the best decision

aid feature for the task (Jones and Schkade 1995; Whitecotton and Butler 1998; Wheeler and

Jones 2003). Further, prior research (e.g., Ghani et al. 2009; Pinsker and Wheeler 2009),

assigned participants one reporting technology rather than directly address the issue of our study:

whether investors will choose an XBRL-enabled or another reporting technology (e.g., Excel or

PDF) to download and analyze financial information. If firms provide their financial information

in XBRL format, but investors choose to analyze the information using other reporting

technologies (e.g., Excel or PDF), then the SEC’s mandate may produce XBRL-formatted

information that investors will not use. Thus, mandatory XBRL adoption may not provide the

expected benefits to investors (see Pinsker and Li 2008 for more details).

We conjecture that nonprofessional investors are likely to choose to use only one

reporting technology when obtaining and analyzing financial information due to time constraints

and the desire to minimize cognitive effort. While the extant user choice literature (Jones and

Schkade 1995; Whitecotton and Butler 1998; Rose 2002; Wheeler and Jones 2003) finds that

2 XBRL supporters include a consortium of over 400 CPA firms, companies (e.g., Microsoft), regulators (e.g., Securities and Exchange Commission), standard setters and accounting bodies (e.g., the Financial Accounting Standards Board, the International Accounting Standards Committee and the Canadian Institute of Chartered Accountants [Trites 1999; Debreceny and Gray 2001; Debreceny et al. 2005]).

Page 5: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

3

users may choose a decision aid feature not created for the specific purpose, little is known

regarding why users may make a choice which appears to be ‘irrational.’ We expand the user

choice literature that examines exclusive choice between alternative decision aid features to

investigate exclusive choice between reporting technologies. Accordingly, we define technology

choice as the decision between alternative technologies.

Further, we develop theoretically-driven explanations as to why users may make these

‘irrational’ choices. Based on existing user choice literature, we hypothesize that nonprofessional

investors will choose a reporting technology due to efficiency issues (i.e., users perceive the task

will take less time to complete).3 Further, we employ cognitive fit theory and the Technology

Acceptance Model (TAM; Davis 1989) to develop two alternative explanation hypotheses. The

first, based on cognitive fit theory, suggests that nonprofessional investors may base their

technology choice on prior experience with that technology. The second, based on TAM,

postulates that nonprofessional investors will choose a reporting technology due to its perceived

usefulness and perceived ease of use.

We examine nonprofessional investors’ choice of three reporting technologies (PDF,

Excel, and XBRL-related) since most U.S. companies’ Investor Relations web sites and the

SEC’s web site present data in formats which are usable by these three reporting technologies.4

We trained participants to use each reporting technology to perform a financial analysis task.

Following training, participants were required to choose one of the three technologies to

complete the financial analysis task. The task consisted of selecting specific items from the

3 As explained in greater detail later in the paper, we examine the impact of technology choice for nonprofessional investors since XBRL proponents argue that nonprofessional investors, rather than professional financial analysts, are most likely to benefit from the technology (Hodge et al. 2004; Baldwin et al. 2006). 4 We acknowledge that these technologies can be used to perform non-financial reporting tasks.

Page 6: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

4

financial statements of two existing companies, calculating three financial ratios to create

investment decision model estimates for each company, and making an investment choice based

on these estimates. Modifying the experimental design used by Hodge et al. (2004), we selected

experimental companies where one company’s net income was significantly higher than the

second company. However, if the participants searched for the required financial statement items

and created the investment decision model estimates correctly, the second company had a better

investment decision model estimate than the first.

Results indicate that more participants (66 percent) chose to use the XBRL-enabled

technology proxy (i.e., EDGAR-Online’s I-Metrix) to complete their financial analysis task

relative to completing the task using a spreadsheet (34 percent). When examining why users

chose a specific reporting technology, we find the participants who chose the XBRL-enabled

reporting technology indicated that the task was more efficient (i.e., took less time) using XBRL-

enabled reporting technology compared to Excel or PDF. Further, participants who chose Excel

indicated that their technology choice was driven by the amount of prior experience with that

technology. Finally, participants did not (1) choose PDF or (2) base their technology choice on

perceived usefulness or perceived ease of use.

Our study makes both academic and practical contributions to the extant research. While

Hodge et al. (2004) found evidence that XBRL-enabled technology provides better data search

functionality, they noted that almost 50 percent of participants did not choose to use XBRL-

enabled technology. Further, Pinsker and Wheeler (2009) report that investors perceive that

XBRL-enabled technology is beneficial; however, they did not directly address the issue of

which reporting technology investors would prefer to choose to complete a financial analysis

task. We extend Hodge et al. (2004) and Pinsker and Wheeler (2009) by (1) designing a study

Page 7: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

5

that directly requires nonprofessional investors to choose between alternative reporting

technologies, and (2) examining why nonprofessional investors choose a particular reporting

technology to complete their financial analysis task. We also extend the extant user choice

literature describing choices between alternative decision aid features (Wheeler and Jones 2003)

to investigate technology choice, that is users’ exclusive choice between alternative technologies.

Our results may be of interest to academics examining technology choice, investor groups and

the SEC who are interested in nonprofessional investor behavior, regulators considering

mandating XBRL reporting requirements, and software designers developing XBRL-enabled

reporting technology for investor use.

II. BACKGROUND AND HYPOTHESIS DEVELOPMENT

XBRL represents a unique financial reporting format relative to previous reporting

formats, offering investors a choice that facilitates searching activities (Hodge et al. 2004).

Clements and Wolfe (2000, 79) observe that “media choice in financial reporting is a new

phenomenon brought on by the widespread use of multimedia-capable computers and financial

reporting on the Internet.” We examine financial report users’ choice of reporting technology for

analyzing financial information. Proponents of XBRL argue that XBRL-tagged financial

statements increase information transparency by allowing users to search for and directly select

the individual financial items they deem to be most important and use these items in financial

analysis tasks without little or no data reentry required (Pinsker and Wheeler 2009; Janvrin and

Mascha 2010). In contrast, investors must sequentially search for specific financial items

reported in Excel and PDF formats and then manually enter this information into their financial

analysis model (Bartley et al. 2011).

Page 8: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

6

Research to date has focused either on the practical benefits of XBRL to users (Baldwin

et al. 2006) or technical aspects of XBRL implementation (Plumlee and Plumlee 2008; Janvrin

and Mascha 2010; Bartley et al. 2011). In addition, prior research and debate throughout the SEC

XBRL mandate implementation process has lamented the slow rate of XBRL-enabled

technology adoption by investors (Cox 2006; Gunn 2007; Troshani and Rao 2007; XSB 2010;

Bartley et al. 2011; Blankespoor et al. 2011). Debreceny et al. (2005) suggest that developing

investor applications for XBRL-formatted documents is important to XBRL’s success. Pinsker

and Wheeler (2009) assigned participants one reporting technology to use (either XBRL-enabled

or paper-based) and found that users perceive that XBRL-enabled reporting technology is

beneficial for financial analysis. Further, Ghani et al. (2009) assigned participants one of three

reporting technologies to use: PDF, HTML, or XBRL-enabled. They find that participants using

XBRL-enabled technology provided higher perceived usefulness ratings than did PDF or HTML

participants; however, perceptions of ease of use were similar across the three reporting

technologies. Finally, the XBRL International Standards Board (2010) recently issued a strategic

initiative to catalyze development of more XBRL-enabled technology.

Unfortunately, prior research (e.g., Ghani et al. 2009; Pinsker and Wheeler 2009) does

not require participants to choose between using XBRL-enabled or other reporting technologies

to perform financial analysis even though many proponents of XBRL argue that nonprofessional

investors will rely exclusively on XBRL-enabled technology instead of spreadsheet or portable

document file technology (Cox 2006; Locke et al. 2009). Thus, practitioners and standard setters

assume that nonprofessional investors will choose XBRL-enabled technology for financial

analysis (Pinsker and Wheeler 2009). Examining technology choice is important since prior

research suggests that user choice may be driven by heuristics that lead to suboptimal

Page 9: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

7

performance (Wheeler and Jones 2003). Further, Hodge et al. (2004) found that nearly 50

percent of their participants did not choose to use XBRL-enabled technology. Therefore, we first

examine:

RQ1: Which reporting technology (PDF, Excel, or XBRL-enabled) will nonprofessional investors choose to complete a complex, financial analysis task?

Our research assumes that investors must make an exclusive choice of reporting

technology due to time constraints and the desire to minimize cognitive effort. That is, the

investors must choose between using XBRL-enabled or another reporting technology typically

available on firms’ Investor Relations web sites (specifically, Excel and PDF) to perform

financial statement analysis. We expand user choice literature used in prior accounting

information systems’ research that examined exclusive choice between alternative decision aid

features (Wheeler and Jones 2003) to investigate technology choice. We define technology

choice as the decision to use one technology to the exclusion of all other alternative reporting

technologies. Further, we develop theoretically-driven explanations to examine why users’ chose

a specific reporting technology for a financial analysis task.

TechnologyChoice

Nonprofessional investors often encounter uncertainty when choosing among reporting

technologies. Early psychological and social science research commonly relied on expected

utility theory to examine decision-making under uncertainty. However, expected utility theory

often failed to predict choice behavior (see Einhorn and Hogarth 1986) since findings from

examining simple gambles in highly-controlled laboratory settings may not generalize to

complicated uncertainties individuals face in real-world settings. In response, Hogarth and Reder

Page 10: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

8

(1987) developed rational choice theory. Rational choice theory relies on rationality to explain

choices and assumes that decision-makers pursue behaviors at their lowest possible costs, given

their beliefs (Pincione and Teson 2006). Yet, multiple studies in varying contexts indicate that

decision-makers did not make rational choices (operationalized as “optimal” choices) due to

ambiguity avoidance (Ellsberg 1961), perceived high domain expertise (Arkes et al. 1986), or

high self-determination (i.e., self-attribution; Becker 1997). These reasons suggest that

individuals make decisions consistent with their own mental models. Thus, individuals may have

difficulty incorporating information (e.g., a new technology) outside their existing mental models

(Lewis et al. 1988).

The user choice literature examining decisions between alternative decision aid features

evolved from rational choice theory. Based on user choice literature, Wheeler and Jones (2003)

find that users provided with alternative decision aid features must engage in some form of

choice behavior. Similarly, to analyze financial information, nonprofessional investors may

choose to rely on (1) one reporting technology to the exclusion of the others; (2) one reporting

technology for one instance of a task, but use other technologies subsequently; or (3) two or

more reporting technologies to varying degrees for the same task (e.g., analyze the same task

separately with each technology). Case studies and discussions with practitioners suggest that

option (2) is unlikely since once most participants adopt a reporting technology, they use it when

completing related instances of the same or similar tasks. Further, prior research indicates that

option (3) is unlikely because of time constraints, the desire to minimize cognitive effort, the

opportunity costs incurred by using additional technologies, and the belief that the use of more

than one reporting technology is unnecessary due to personal competence (Wheeler and Jones

2003). Consequently, we examine option (1).

Page 11: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

9

Dos Santos and Bariff (1988, 461) state that there exists “the common assumption that

greater flexibility and choice in software aids will promote improved user performance.”

However, research does not fully support this prediction. Wheeler and Jones (2003) found that

user choice systematically resulted in sub-optimal decisions, because many users’ chose not to

use the decision aid feature. Rather, users evoke heuristics and over-attribute their own abilities

as a means to circumvent the aid. Whitecotton and Butler (1998) find similar results.5

Heuristics also play a role in the relationship among problem representation, cognitive

effort, and perceived task efficiency. For example, Hoch and Schkade (1996) find evidence that

users choose the technology that most efficiently utilizes their cognitive ability in a demanding

environment. More specifically, Jones and Schkade (1995) state that decision makers will apply

the representativeness heuristic when they encounter an unknown or unfamiliar problem

format/representation if they perceive that doing so outweighs the cost of increased cognitive

effort. The representativeness heuristic helps the decision makers reformat the problem into a

more familiar or representative mental model, which increases task efficiency.

Although the extant user choice literature finds uncertainty with regard to the relationship

between choice and performance (Wheeler and Jones 2003), the representative heuristic noted in

Jones and Schkade (1995) suggests that perceived user efficiency gains play a role in technology

choice. Thus, decision makers are more likely to invoke the representativeness heuristic and

choose the technology which they perceive can help them complete the task more efficiently by

reducing time needed to perform the task. Accordingly, we propose the following hypothesis.

5 A robust finding in decision aid research is that users rarely rely on the decision aid in an optimal manner, systematically either under-relying or over-relying (cf. Rose 2002 for review).

Page 12: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

10

H1: The reporting technology nonprofessional investors choose to complete the financial analysis task will be directly related to perceived task efficiency (i.e., perceived less time to complete the task).

The user choice literature cited to support hypothesis one represents one potential

explanation for technology choice. Yet, this literature is still being developed and is fairly narrow

in scope. Specifically, the prior user choice literature is restricted to investigating (1) whether or

not technology, in the form of features within a researcher-created decision aid, would be

chosen, and (2) if users would rely on the decision aid feature to form a decision. Consequently,

we look to other potentially-related information systems theories, specifically cognitive fit and

TAM, to develop additional explanations regarding why users choose a particular reporting

technology.

Cognitive Fit Theory

Cognitive fit theory suggests that task effectiveness increases as the three-way match

among (1) the problem representation, (2) the problem-solving task, and (3) the users’ problem-

solving skill set increases (Vessey 1991; Vessey and Galletta 1991). The theory suggests that

decision makers correctly perceive improvements in cognitive fit (Vessey and Galletta 1991;

Vessey 2006). Prior psychology literature finds that users may not select decision rules that

would allow them to choose correctly on 70 percent of judgment tasks (Arkes et al. 1986).

Specifically related to technology usage, Hayes (2004) notes that users typically resist giving up

software they are experienced with to learn new software, even though the new software is

perceived to be better. Thus, we argue that prior experience with a technology represents a

potential explanation for technology choice. Our argument is supported by Jones and Schkade’s

(1995) results indicating that decision makers tend to choose the problem representation with

Page 13: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

11

which they have the most experience and to a lesser extent by Arkes et al.(1986)’s finding that

those with perceived higher levels of domain expertise tend not to use a given decision rule.

Further, the cognitive effort literature (e.g., Jones and Schkade 1995) suggests that users

choose the problem representation with which they are most experienced. Prior experience is a

heuristic in its own right, because decision makers know they can rely on it to make decisions

and save cognitive effort. Decision makers have a choice to either expend cognitive effort to

reformat a new problem representation into their mental models or not. Consistent with research

examining cognitive fit and cognitive effort, we expect that some decision makers will choose to

not exert the necessary cognitive effort to reformat the problem representation into their mental

models, and instead base their technology choice on prior experience.6

H2: The reporting technology nonprofessional investors choose to complete the financial analysis task will be directly related to their prior experience with that technology.

The Technology Acceptance Model

TAM postulates that when users are provided new technology (e.g., XBRL-enabled

technology) to assist them in performing assigned tasks, their perceptions of the technology’s

usefulness and ease of use significantly influence their acceptance and assumed usage of the

technology (Davis et al. 1989; Bagozzi et al. 1992). Perceived usefulness is the degree to which a

person believes that using a technology could enhance his or her job performance, while

perceived ease of use is the degree to which a person believes that using a technology could be

6 While we acknowledge that some prior studies include experience as a covariate (e.g., Pinsker and Wheeler 2009), cognitive fit theory and our unique research design identify experience as a variable pertinent to our research question.

Page 14: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

12

free from effort (Davis et al. 1989). However, TAM does not postulate that a technology’s

functional superiority to other technologies will necessarily result in its perception by users as

superior. In fact, the literature indicates that under-utilization of technology due to users’

negative misperceptions is a major problem in business (Mun and Hwang 2003).

TAM research and related findings have important implications for technology choice.

Pinsker and Wheeler (2009) find a difference in favorability perceptions (relating both to their

own analysis and to the firm providing XBRL-enabled financials) between XBRL-enabled users

and PDF users. Investigating the perceptions of information technology users is important when

examining expectations about the diffusion of technology into the business community, since

TAM research attempts to link user perceptions of technology to technology use (Pinsker and

Wheeler 2009).

Our reporting technology choices allow users to search and retrieve financial information,

but at different thresholds (i.e., XBRL’s drill-down possibilities are at a more detailed level than

Excel or PDF). Therefore, we expect that technology choice should be related ot the level of

perceived usefulness. Further, our reporting technology choices are likely to result in higher

perceived ease of use, because all three choices (PDF, Excel, and XBRL-enabled) allow

nonprofessional investors the ability to gather, integrate, and compare firm data more rapidly,

and therefore, at a lower cost, compared to using paper-based data. We propose the following

hypotheses.

H3a: The reporting technology nonprofessional investors choose to complete the financial analysis task will be directly related to perceived usefulness. H3b: The reporting technology nonprofessional investors choose to complete the financial analysis task will be directly related to perceived ease of use.

Page 15: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

13

III. METHOD

Participants

We focus on technology choices made by nonprofessional rather than professional

investors since prior research suggests that search-facilitating technology such as XBRL will

most likely benefit nonprofessional investors. Specifically, prior research documents that

experienced financial analysts generally possess well-defined valuation models and search for

specific financial statements items of interest (Frederickson and Miller 2004; Hunton and

McEwen 1997). In contrast, nonprofessional investors are more likely to possess less developed

valuation models and sequentially search for financial statement information (Hunton and

McEwen 1997; Frederickson and Miller 2004). Thus, nonprofessional investors are more likely

to benefit from using reporting technologies that may facilitate searching activities.

Our nonprofessional investor participants were 53 graduate business students enrolled in

a financial statement analysis course at two medium-sized state universities. Each participant

received course credit for completing the experiment. Table 1 provides descriptive statistics for

our sample. Libby et al. (2002) contend that experiments that focus on the judgments of

nonprofessional investors only require participants who possess basic accounting and investing

knowledge. Our student participants had completed at least two undergraduate financial

accounting courses (not displayed in the table) and over 30 percent had previously bought or sold

common stock or mutual funds. Further, two-thirds of the participants intend on buying stock in

the next five years. On average, participants had analyzed financial statements over five times.

Thus, these demographics suggest that the participants had the requisite course and “hands-on”

experience to perform the financial analysis task.

[Insert Table 1 about here]

Page 16: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

14

Procedures and Financial Analysis Task

We used software to conduct the experiment online. As illustrated in Figure 1, the first

screen in phase I provided a brief introduction to the experimental task. Participants then

navigated to an overview of the task and three reporting technologies: PDF, Excel, and XBRL-

enabled.7 Next, the participants were trained to use each reporting technology to complete the

financial analysis task. The financial analysis task involved creating investment decision model

estimates for two companies.8 Similar to Wheeler and Arunachalam (2008), the investment

decision model included three common financial ratios. To create each investment decision

model, participants were required to search for selected items from each company’s financial

statements and use these items to calculate price-to-book, revenue to assets, and operating

income margin ratios. Participants were given weights to apply to each ratio to form their

investment decision models and asked to determine which of the two companies they would

invest in.

[Insert Figure 1 – Experimental Procedure – about here]

The training guided participants through step-by-step instructions of how to use each

reporting technology to calculate the investment decision model for two sample firms, Abaxis

and Blackbaud. All participants viewed the same training tasks although the order of presentation

7 We used EDGAR-Online’s I-Metrix tool to proxy for XBRL-enabled technology. To ensure that our task consisted of a test of the XBRL-enabled technology in general, rather than a test of the specific tool we chose, our task was designed to be completed using many of the XBRL-enabled tools currently available in the marketplace. Further, we pilot tested the experiment using both I-Metrix and Savanet tools to proxy for XBRL-enabled technology and found no significant differences in our results. 8 Consistent with Hodge et al. (2004), we define our task as “complex,” because participants were required to acquire information from financial statements, integrate this information into investment decision models and use the results to form investment judgments.

Page 17: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

15

(PDF, Excel, and XBRL-enabled) was randomized. Participants could go back and retrain if they

felt uncomfortable with any reporting technology.

In phase II, participants navigated to an overview screen, which presented summary

financial statement information for two new companies, Concur Technologies and CommVault,

in a neutral Word format.9 Participants were asked to review the summary financial information

and make judgments regarding which of these companies to invest in. Next, participants were

asked to calculate the investment decision models for each company using only one of the three

reporting technologies. This technology choice decision represents the dependent variable

TECHNOLOGY CHOICE. Once participants made their technology choice, the software

prevented them from backtracking and changing their choice. After calculating and recording the

investment decision model estimates for both companies, participants indicated which company

they would prefer to invest in. Modifying the experimental design used by Hodge et al. (2004),

we designed the experiment so that one company’s reported net income was higher while the

other company had a higher (i.e., better) investment decision model estimate.

In phase III, participants completed a post-test questionnaire containing demographic

questions (including prior experience with PDF, Excel, and XBRL-enabled technologies; TAM

questions on perceived usefulness and perceived ease of use, and both open- and close-ended

questions on why the participants chose the particular reporting technology).

Some participants completed the task in the classroom as part of a three-hour, ‘in-lab’

class, while others completed the task outside the class to simulate a typical day-trader. Chi-

9 We chose two unfamiliar companies for the training and two other unfamiliar companies for the actual test in order to avoid familiarity demand effects. Results from demographic questions suggest that participants had no familiarity with either Concur or CommVault.

Page 18: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

16

square tests did not show any differences between TECHNOLOGY CHOICE responses between

in-lab and out-of-lab participants (χ2 = 0.03; p-value = 0.87). The in-lab participants took an

average of approximately 45 minutes to complete the entire experiment with a range of 35

minutes to approximately 90 minutes. We were unable to collect this information for the out-of-

lab participants, but do not believe it is a threat to our research design given the insignificant

difference between groups reported above.

Pilot Studies

Using both graduate and undergraduate students, we conducted two years of pilot testing

in order to examine many factors: clarity of the research instrument; understandability and

appropriateness of single-item constructs; time of task; task complexity; generalizability of task

to multiple XBRL-enabled tools; and familiarity with sample companies and technologies

employed. Based on feedback obtained, we attempted to equalize the amount of time necessary

to train on each technology. All three reporting technologies took approximately the same

amount of time for the participants to complete the training, but the XBRL-enabled technology

took up more screen space than the others due to including the requisite number of screen

shots.10 We believe this slight difference is realistic given the relative newness of XBRL

(compared to the other two reporting technologies) and its software tools.

Additional pilot testing included the administration of surveys in order to collect more

process-oriented data. Using a series of t-tests and separating participants based on which

reporting technology they chose, the survey data did not find significant differences (using an

alpha of five percent) between XBRL-enabled technology and Excel choosers with regard to

10 Initially, our design included both a simple task (calculating the current ratio) and a complex task (the current study’s task). Pretests showed an average completion time of approximately 90 minutes; thus, due to time constraints, we decided to include only the more realistic financial analysis task for this study.

Page 19: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

17

perceived complexity, length, or confusion of the experimental directions for any of the reporting

technologies. Pilot study survey data also allowed us to be more precise in operationalizing our

explanatory variables.

Dependent Variable

Similar to related user choice research (Wheeler and Jones 2003), we use

TECHNOLOGY CHOICE as our dependent variable. To operationalize TECHNOLOGY

CHOICE, participants were required to choose one reporting technology (PDF, Excel, or XBRL-

enabled) to use to complete the financial analysis task following training for all three reporting

technologies. We chose these three reporting technologies since several U.S. firms’ Investor

Relations web sites (e.g., Microsoft [2011]) and the SEC’s web site allow users to download

financial information in applicable formats. We coded TECHNOLOGY CHOICE as follows: 0 =

participant chose Excel; 1 = participant chose XBRL-enabled; and 2 = participant chose PDF.

Explanatory Variables

As noted above, our design consisted of training in three reporting technologies, followed

by the experimental task of choosing one technology to perform a financial analysis task. All

participants viewed the same training tasks. Based on theory and our pilot study results, our

explanatory variables consisted of the potential explanations why participants would choose a

particular technology for their financial analysis task: efficiency (i.e., takes less time to complete

task), prior experience, perceived usefulness and perceived ease of use.11 Each variable was

measured on an 11-point scale (0-10) with higher scores indicating higher perceived importance.

11 We also included two other potential explanations for choosing a reporting technology for the task: (1) “because the researchers wanted me to choose it” and (2) “because I thought it was the latest state-of-the-art technology.” Both relate to potential demand effects in the experiment, but neither was statistically significant in our upcoming analyses. Thus, we do not discuss them further.

Page 20: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

18

Given the uniqueness of a choice setting relative to experimenter-assigned conditions, we

were careful to tie our operationalizations of explanatory variables closely to the most applicable

prior research studies. For instance, we operationalized efficiency as taking less time to complete

the task in order to be consistent with related financial analysis (Hodge et al. 2004) and user

choice (Wheeler and Jones 2003) research. The participants’ prior experience with each

reporting technology was measured consistent with Hoch and Schkade (1996) incorporating 11-

point scales ranging from “novice” to “expert.” For the TAM constructs of perceived usefuleness

and perceived ease of use, we adopt single-item measures consistent with Pinsker and Wheeler

(2009), rather than using the scales developed in Davis’ (1989) seminal work. Single-item (rather

than multiple-item) measures are appropriate when the constructs measured are understandable

per se by participants (Ilgen et al. 1981; Wanous et al. 1997).12 Given the previously-cited pilot

tests, open-ended responses as to why a particular reporting technology was chosen, and the

participants’ self-reported experience with examining firms’ financials (presented in Table 1), we

believe that our single-item TAM measures are appropriate and sufficiently unambiguous to

participants.

IV. RESULTS

Manipulation Checks

Two manipulation check questions were used. The first question asked whether the

participants understood that they had a choice of reporting technology to use to complete the

12 According to Pinsker and Wheeler (2009, 261), prior research notes multi-dimensional constructs (e.g., “personality factors”) as those being too complex for single-item measures. These constructs are typically misunderstood by participants and involve processes of which the participants are unaware. Constructs for which prior research has empirically verified to be appropriate include “job satisfaction” and “probability that an effort affects performance of a task.” These latter constructs have unambiguous meanings to participants and participants are fully aware of their beliefs and perceptions regarding them.

Page 21: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

19

investment task (after the training phase). Using a binary “True/False” response, participants

responded to the following statement in the post-test questionnaire, “In the second part of the

study, I was allowed to choose whether I wanted to perform the task using Excel-only, PDF, or I-

Metrix (please respond by choosing True or False).”13 Forty-nine out of 53 participants (92.5

percent) correctly responded “True.” We conclude, therefore, that participants properly

understood they could choose one of three technologies to complete the task.14

The second item was a statement related to understanding the type of task performed.

Specifically, the statement read, “I was asked to make investment judgments.” All 53

participants correctly answered “True.” Thus, the participants appeared to understand the task at

hand. In sum, both manipulation check questions were successful.15

Research Question

We begin our tests by investigating our research question: which reporting technology do

nonprofessional investors’ choose to complete the complex, financial analysis task? Using our

dependent variable, TECHNOLOGY CHOICE, we explored whether one reporting technology

dominated participants’ choice. Thirty-five of 53 participants (66 percent) chose XBRL-enabled

technology (I-Metrix) to perform their task; 18 chose Excel (34 percent), and none chose PDF. A

13 We note that I-Metrix uses Excel sparingly as part of its interface. Thus, we clearly specified Excel on its own versus XBRL-enabled (I-Metrix and its occasional Excel interface) in all experimental materials. 14 Removing the four participants who did not answer this question correctly did not materially affect our results. Thus, we include these participants in our analyses. 15 We also inquired as to the salience of the course credit and if the participants could identify that I-Metrix was an XBRL-enabled reporting technology. Only one participant (1.9 percent) did not respond correctly about receiving course credit. Forty-six participants (86.8 percent) correctly identified I-Metrix as an XBRL-enabled decision-making technology, rather than a database, spreadsheet, or word processor. Removing the one incorrect respondent from the course credit questionnaire item or the seven incorrect respondents on the I-Metrix item did not materially change our results. Thus, we include all 53 participants in our analyses.

Page 22: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

20

Chi-Square test shows a critical majority of participants favored the XBRL-enabled technology

relative to Excel (χ2 = 5.45, p-value = 0.02).

Next, we examine why nonprofessional investors’ chose a certain reporting technology

(Excel or XBRL-enabled). We examine the correlations between our four explanatory variables

and TECHNOLOGY CHOICE (see Table 2) to gain an initial understanding of the data.16

Examining Table 2, and using one-tailed tests we find that PRIOR EXPERIENCE is

significantly correlated with TECHNOLOGY CHOICE (corr. = -0.48, p-value < 0.01). Given

our dependent variable coding described earlier, we interpret the negative correlation to suggest

that participants who chose Excel (coded as zero) are more likely to base their decision on prior

experience with the reporting technology than are participants who chose XBRL-enabled

technology. The only other significant explanatory variable was TAKES LESS TIME (corr. =

0.26, p-value < 0.05). Given our coding, we interpret this correlation to suggest that participants

who chose XBRL-enabled technology (coded as one) perceive that they could perform the task

in less time using XBRL-enabled technology than using either PDF or Excel. Neither TAM

construct was correlated with TECHNOLOGY CHOICE.

[Insert Table 2 about here]

Hypothesis Testing

We employ Kruskal-Wallis (KW) to test our hypotheses. The KW test rank-orders the

technology choice explanatory variables to provide insights regarding why nonprofessional

16 Unless otherwise specified, we use two-tailed tests. We also investigated the effect of demographic differences identified in Table 1, with the exception of the last two items related to task realism and task difficulty, as possible covariates in our analyses. Excluding the PRIOR EXPERIENCE variables, no demographic characteristics were significant at a p-value < 0.10. Additionally, we considered risk preference as a factor in technology choice. We used three questions adapted from Kahneman and Tversky (1979), but they did not correlate well together (Cronbach’s Alpha = 0.44), and did not clearly affect TECHNOLOGY CHOICE.

Page 23: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

21

investors chose their particular reporting technology. H1 uses extant user choice literature to

predict that participants will choose a reporting technology based on efficiency (time) issues.

Examining Table 3, TAKES LESS TIME has significantly different mean ranks (mean rank of

the Excel choice = 21.33; mean rank of the XBRL-enabled technology choice = 29.91; χ2 = 3.80,

p-value < 0.05); thus supporting H1. Thus, we find that the mean ranks are higher for the XBRL-

enabled choosers (the majority reporting technology chosen). Therefore, we provide evidence

that TAKES LESS TIME is the main factor determining TECHNOLOGY CHOICE for XBRL-

enabled choosers.

H2 incorporates cognitive fit theory to predict that TECHNOLOGY CHOICE will be

directly related to PRIOR EXPERIENCE. As shown in Table 3, the mean ranks for PRIOR

EXPERIENCE are significantly different (mean rank of the Excel choice = 37.81; mean rank of

the XBRL-enabled technology choice = 21.44; χ2 = 13.51, p-value < 0.001); supporting H2.

Thus, our results suggest that for the Excel choosers, PRIOR EXPERIENCE is the main

explanation for their reporting technology choice.

Lastly, H3a and H3b predicted that TECHNOLOGY CHOICE will be directly related to

a reporting technology’s perceived usefulness and perceived ease of use, respectively. Results

shown in Table 3 indicate that neither of the mean rank differences for these TAM variables are

significantly different (for PERCEIVED USEFULNESS [mean rank of the Excel choice = 22.83;

mean rank of the XBRL-enabled technology choice = 29.14; χ2 = 2.05, p-value = 0.15] and

PERCEIVED EASE OF USE [mean rank of the Excel choice = 23.92; mean rank of the XBRL-

enabled technology choice = 28.59; χ2 = 1.11, p-value = 0.29]). This evidence rejects both H3a

Page 24: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

22

and H3b and suggests that the TAM measures are not appropriate explanatory variables of

TECHNOLOGY CHOICE.17

[Insert Table 3 about here]

Further Analysis

Single Explanation for Technology Choice

We asked our participants to identify which explanatory variable (TAKES LESS TIME,

PRIOR EXPERIENCE, PERCEIVED USEFULNESS, or PERCEIVED EASE OF USE) was

their most important explanation for their reporting technology choice. There were no significant

differences among the top two explanations chosen (TAKES LESS TIME and PERCEIVED

USEFULNESS; χ2 = 0.01, p-value = 0.94). We then separated the responses by technology

choice (i.e., Excel vs. XBRL-enabled technology) and found mixed results. Specifically, even

though the Excel choosers selected PRIOR EXPERIENCE as their most important explanation

most often (eight times relative to only three times for the next highest selection choice), it was

not statistically significantly higher than the other choices (χ2 = 7.56, p-value = 0.11). Despite

not being able to distinguish the XBRL-enabled technology choosers most common explanation,

we find that PRIOR EXPERIENCE represented the least important explanation (only two

17 As a supplemental analysis, we examined the technology choice explanations using principal component analysis. Our initial analysis revealed six factors with eigenvalues greater than one, but no clear pattern regarding factor loadings. Therefore, we employed Varimax and Promax rotation method. Both rotations resulted in three factors, but the Promax rotation, being oblique, provided greater differences between variable loadings (SAS 2005). PERCEIVED USEFULENESS, PERCEIVED EASE OF USE, and TAKES LESS TIME each load on one factor which we refer to as EFFICIENCY (0.46, 1.00, and 0.99, respectively). PRIOR EXPERIENCE loads almost exclusively (1.00) on a second factor. Finally, confirming the earlier non-significant results noted in footnote #11 and our decision to exclude from subsequent analysis, the explanations ‘researcher wanted me to choose it’ and ‘latest state-of-the-art technology’ load on third factor (0.71 and 1.00 respectively). Thus, principal component analysis provides results consistent with our main analyses.

Page 25: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

23

participants chose this explanation; χ2 = 11.29, p-value = 0.01). Thus, we provide weak

incremental evidence to supplement the hypothesis testing results.

Relating the Technology Choice to Investment Choice

Modifying the experimental design used by Hodge et al. (2004), we chose our sample

companies such that one company seemed to be the “better” investment choice when considering

only the face financials, but the other company would be chosen if the participants correctly

calculated and relied upon the investment decision model (a proxy for accuracy). The experiment

was run over two years with different graduate business students and we used actual firm data

reported using the actual reporting technologies employed by the companies. Interestingly, the

companies reversed as to the “correct” company to invest in (using the investment decision

model estimates). In the first year, CommVault had the “better” financials, but Concur

Technologies had the higher investment decision model estimate. The opposite was true in the

second year. Thus, this reversal of firm performance biases us against finding an effect.

Rather than only coding the individual companies, we used a binary coding of 0 = the

“incorrect company” and 1 = the “correct” company to invest in given the investor decision

model estimates. Twenty-four participants chose the “correct” company to invest in; 24 chose the

“incorrect” company; five participants did not provide a choice. Chi-Square testing (χ2 = 0.87; p-

value = 0.35) shows no relationship between TECHNOLOGY CHOICE and choosing the

“correct” company (i.e., no accuracy differences in favor of a given technology).18

18 Since we initially asked participants to choose a company solely given their financial data, we attempted to link this initial decision to TECHNOLOGY CHOICE. A Chi-Square test indicated no statistical difference in initial accuracy choice between those choosing XBRL-enabled and Excel (χ2 = 1.22, p-value = 0.27). Further testing did not find any accuracy differences for those who changed their mind relative to participants who did not change their mind from their initial company chosen (χ2 = 0.43, p-value = 0.51) or when comparing participants who changed from an incorrect (correct) initial company choice to a correct (incorrect) final investment company choice (χ2 = 0.38, p-value = 0.83).

Page 26: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

24

Investment Decision Model and Acquisition Technology

We explored the perceived importance (or lack thereof) of TECHNOLOGY CHOICE on

the investment decision model. We used an 11-point scale (0-10) with the higher scores meaning

higher perceived importance. Using a t-test, we found that the mean of the Excel choosers (7.17,

S.D. = 1.25) was marginally higher than the XBRL-enabled technology choosers (6.23, S.D. =

1.86; t = 1.92, p-value = 0.06). Therefore, we find only weak evidence linking the importance of

reporting technology choice to the investor decision model.19

Next, motivated by Hodge et al. (2004)’s argument that XBRL-formatted data will

facilitate information acquisition, we asked participants which reporting technology (PDF, Excel,

or XBRL-enabled) they would prefer to use to acquire a company’s financial information. Thirty

participants chose XBRL-enabled (56.6 percent), 21 chose Excel (39.6 percent), and two chose

PDF (3.8 percent). A Chi-Square test did not show any significant differences between XBRL-

enabled and Excel choices for acquiring financial information (χ2 = 1.59, p-value = 0.21).

Prior Technology Experience

Our hypothesis testing indicated that PRIOR EXPERIENCE was the key factor as to why

a minority of participants chose Excel over XBRL-enabled technology in their financial analysis

task. We performed an additional paired-samples t-test to provide empirical support for this

finding. We examined the participants’ reported prior experiences with both Excel and XBRL-

enabled reporting technologies as presented in Table 1. Both of these measurements were on 11- 19 Further, we asked participants to confirm which reporting technology chosen when calculating the investment decision model, The participants’ choices included paper-and-pencil, calculator, spreadsheet (Excel), mental methods, I-Metrix (XBRL), PDF, and other. Only 22 participants responded to this question. Of the 22 respondents, 11 chose spreadsheet and nine chose I-Metrix, with one answer each for calculator and PDF. These descriptive results add credence to the dominant choices of spreadsheet and XBRL-enabled technology found in the hypothesis testing.

Page 27: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

25

point scales (0-10) with higher scores indicating higher levels of experience. Results indicated

that the participants’ mean PRIOR EXPERIENCE with Excel (7.21, S.D. = 1.92) was

significantly higher than their mean PRIOR EXPERIENCE with XBRL-enabled technology

(3.38, S.D. = 2.90; t-statistic = 27.40, p-value < 0.001). These results supplement our H2 testing.

V. CONCLUSION

Slow acceptance of XBRL-enabled technology has prompted recent regulatory mandates

(e.g., the SEC) designed to assist both the regulators themselves and relatively less sophisticated

market participants. Accordingly, we use an exclusive choice experimental design to examine

whether nonprofessional investors will choose XBRL-enabled or another reporting technology to

complete a financial statement analysis task. We also extend the extant user choice literature to

introduce the concept of technology choice and provide specific explanations why

nonprofessional investors choose to use XBRL-related or an alternative reporting technology

when performing financial analyses. Our work is important since recent evidence suggests

investor adoption of XBRL-enabled technology is slow despite the regulation requirements (SEC

2009) and emphasis on XBRL-formatted information (Hodge et al. 2004; Locke et al. 2009;

Bartley et al. 2011). For example, almost 50 percent of participants in Hodge et al. (2004)

research did not use XBRL-enabled reporting technology. Furthermore, some U.S. firms view

XBRL implementation as a compliance exercise and question the benefits of providing XBRL-

formatted statements (Janvrin and No 2011). In other words, XBRL and its underlying

technology may be under-utilized in terms of its main intended purposes: increased efficiency,

increased reusability, and increased transparency of data (SEC 2009; Bartley et al. 2011;

Vasarhelyi et al. 2011).

Page 28: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

26

We trained nonprofessional investors to use three reporting technologies available to

analyze data formats commonly found on both U.S. public companies’ Investor Relations’

websites and the SEC’s own site (PDF, Excel, and XBRL-enabled) to perform a financial

analysis task. Using an exclusive choice experimental design, we then asked participants to

choose which reporting technology they would use to complete the same financial analysis task

for two different companies. Results indicate 66 percent of participants chose to use XBRL-

enabled technology while 34 percent chose spreadsheets (i.e., Excel). Further, no participants

chose document exchange software (i.e., PDF).

Results from hypothesis testing provide evidence that: (1) efficiency in performing the

task (i.e., takes less time) was the major factor for participants who chose the XBRL-enabled

technology and (2) prior experience with the reporting technology was the major factor for

participants who chose Excel. Our findings suggest that investors are likely to choose XBRL-

enabled technology due to efficiency issues; a positive sign for XBRL advocates.

We do not find empirical support linking the TAM measures of perceived usefulness and

perceived ease of use to technology choice. Prior TAM literature (e.g., Davis 1989, etc.) does not

consider choice in the decision making process. Our results suggest that acceptance (the typical

TAM dependent variable) may not imply usage even though prior research has assumed this to

be the case (i.e., Pinsker and Wheeler 2009). Thus, acceptance may be a requisite, but not

sufficient factor of choice.

Further, we do not find a single explanation for reporting technology choice. Specifically,

spreadsheet choosers’ documented prior experience with the technology as the dominant factor,

but not sufficiently enough to be statistically more relevant than the other explanations; while the

only conclusion from the XBRL-enabled choosers was that prior experience was not a factor.

Page 29: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

27

Our lack of a single explanation finding provides future researchers an opportunity to re-examine

relevant theory and identify other variables that may impact technology choice. We also do not

find any accuracy differences between the reporting technologies chosen. Future research is

needed to validate potential accuracy measures.

Our findings build upon the existing user choice literature that investigates choice

between alternative decision aid features to examine technology choice. We define technology

choice as choice between alternative (reporting) technologies. Further, we develop theoretically-

based explanations as to why individuals chose the specific reporting technology. We argue that

examining technology choice in the context of nonprofessional investors’ choice between using

XBRL-enabled technology relative to other reporting technologies is appropriate since

proponents of XBRL-enabled technology suggest that investors will use it exclusively instead of

other reporting technologies.

As with all experimental work, we acknowledge research limitations. Several limitations

may suggest future research opportunities. First, our participants were required to choose one of

three reporting technologies. While we believe we have chosen the three most common

technologies to display/report financial information, we may have missed a technology. Second,

while we pilot tested our experiment using both Savanet and I-Metrix as our proxies for XBRL-

enabled technology, we used only I-Metrix which provides users with additional financial

statement items not available via XBRL for the actual experiment. Third, our experiment

assumes a cost-free investing environment. Whereas nonprofessional investors may already have

access to PDF and Excel technology, they may need to purchase XBRL-enabled tools. This

consideration was not part of our research design, but represents an interesting avenue for future

research. Fourth, due to time constraints, we use a single task to measure nonprofessional

Page 30: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

28

investor technology choice. Future research should consider multiple tasks of varying complexity

to determine if our results generalize or if reporting technology choice rationale varies by task

complexity. Finally, Lowe and Locke (2011) argue that tagging individual financial statement

items with XBRL tags will allow investors to view the financial items outside the underlying

financial statement format and its related linkages.20 Our financial analysis task assumed

participants had a working knowledge of the underlying financial statement format. Future

research could examine exclusive technology choice for tasks where financial items are

presented in ways that do not require knowledge of the financial statement format.

20 For example, when viewing long term debt securities separately in an XBRL-enabled technology, investors do not automatically view the reference point indicating the total amount of long term liabilities that would be shown if the long-term debt securities amount was presented in a financial statement format.

Page 31: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

29

REFERENCES

Arkes. H. R., R. M. Dawes, and C. Christensen. 1986. Factors influencing the use of a decision rule in a probabilistic task. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 37: 93-110.

Bagozzi, R. P., F. D. Davis, and P. R. Warshaw. 1992. Development and test of a theory of technological learning and usage. Human Relations 45 (7): 660-686.

Baldwin, A. A., C. E. Brown, and B. S. Trinkle. 2006. XBRL: An impacts framework and

research challenge. Journal of Emerging Technologies in Accounting 3 (1): 97-116. Bartley, J. W., A. Y. S. Chen, and E. Z. Taylor. 2011. A comparison of XBRL filings to

corporate 10-Ks – Evidence from the voluntary filing program. Accounting Horizons 25 (2): forthcoming.

Becker, D. A. 1997. The effects of choice on auditors’ intrinsic motivation and performance. Behavioral Research in Accounting 9: 1-19. Blankespoor, E., B. P. Miller, H. D. White. 2011. The initial impact of mandatory search-

facilitating technology: Evidence from the XBRL mandate. Working paper, University of Michigan and Indiana University.

Clements, C. E., and C. J. Wolfe. 2000. Reporting financial results with the video medium: An

experimental analysis. Journal of Information Systems 14 (2): 79-94. Cox, C. 2006. The interactive data revolution: Improved disclosure for investors, less expensive

reporting for companies. Available at: http://www.sec.gov/news/speech/2006/spch053006cc.htm.

Davis, F. D. 1989. Perceived usefulness, perceived ease of use, and user acceptance of

information technology. MIS Quarterly 13 (3): 319-340. _______, R. P. Bagozzi, and P. R. Warshaw. 1989. User acceptance of computer technology:

A comparison of two theoretical models. Management Science 35: 982-1003. Debreceny, R., and G. L. Gray. 2001. The production and use of semantically rich accounting

reports on the Internet XML and XBRL. International Journal of Accounting Information Systems 2 (1): 47-74.

_______, A. Chandra, J. Cheh, D. Guithues-Amrhein, N. Hannon, P. Hutchison, D. Janvrin, R. Jones, B. Lamberton, A. Lymer, M. Mascha, R. Nehmer, S. Roohani, R. Srivastava, S. Trabelsi, T. Tribunella, G. Trites, and M. Vasarhelyi. 2005. Financial reporting in XBRL on the SEC’s EDGAR system: A critique and evaluation. Journal of Information Systems 19 (2): 191-210.

Page 32: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

30

DeLong, J., A. Shleifer, L. Summers, and R. Waldman. 1989. The size and incidence of the

losses from noise trading. Journal of Finance 44: 681-696. Dos Santos, B. L., and M. L. Bariff. 1988. A study of user interface aids for model-oriented

decision support systems. Management Science 34 (4): 461-468. Einhorn, H. J., and R. M. Hogarth. 1986. Decision making under ambiguity. Journal of Business 59 (4): S225-S250. Ellsberg, D. 1961. Risk, ambiguity, and the Savage axioms. Quarterly Journal of Economics 75: 643-669. Frederickson, J., and J. Miller. 2004. The effects of pro forma earnings disclosures on analysts’

and nonprofessional investors’ equity valuation judgments. The Accounting Review 79 (3): 667-686.

Ghani, E.K., F. Laswad, and S. Tooley. 2009. Digital reporting formats: Users’ perceptions,

preferences and performances. International Journal of Digital Accounting Research 9: 45-98.

Gunn, J. 2007. XBRL: Opportunities and challenges in enhancing financial reporting and

assurance processes. Current Issues in Auditing 1 (1): A36-A43. Hayes, F. 2004. Chaos is back. ComputerWorld November 8. Hoch, S. J., and D. A. Schkade. 1996. A psychological approach to decision support systems. Management Science 42 (1): 51-64. Hodge, F. D., J. J. Kennedy, and L. A. Maines. 2004. Does search-facilitating technology

improve the transparency of financial reporting? The Accounting Review 79 (3): 687-703.

Hogarth, R. M., and M. Reder. 1987. Rational Choice. University of Chicago press.

Hunton, J., and R. McEwen. 1997. An assessment of the relation between analysts’ earnings forecast accuracy, motivational incentives and cognitive information search strategy. The Accounting Review 72 (4): 497-0515.

Ilgen, D., D. Nebeker, and R. Pritchard. 1981. Expectancy theory measures: An empirical comparison in an experimental simulation. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance 28: 189-223. Janvrin, D., and M. F. Mascha. 2010. The process of creating XBRL instance documents: A

research framework. Review of Business Information Systems 14 (2): 11-34.

Page 33: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

31

_______, and W. G. No. 2011. XBRL implementation: A field investigation. Working paper,

Iowa State University. Jones, D. R., and D. A. Schkade. 1995. Choosing and translating between problem

representations. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 61 (2): 214-223.

Kahneman, D., and A. Tversky. 1979. Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica 47 (2): 263-291. Lewis, B. L., J. M. Patton, and S. L. Green. 1988. The effects of information choice and

information use on analysts’ predictions of municipal bond rating changes. The Accounting Review 63 (2): 270-282.

Libby, R., R. Bloomfield, and M. W. Nelson. 2002. Experimental research in financial accounting. Accounting, Organizations & Society 27 (8): 775-810.

Locke, J., A. Lowe, and A. Lymer. 2009. Sociotechnical alignment within the interactive data

constituency. Working paper, University of Birmingham, U.K. Lowe, A, and J. Locke. 2011. Digital business reporting: Epistemic culture clash for

accountants. Working paper, Aston University and University of Birmingham. Maudlin, E. G., and L. V. Ruchala. 1999. Towards a meta-theory of accounting information systems. Accounting, Organizations & Society, 24 (4): 317-331. Mircosoft. 2011. Investor Relations. Available at:

http://www.microsoft.com/investor/SEC/default.aspx. Mun, Y., and Y. Hwang. 2003. Predicting the use of web-based information systems: Self-

efficacy, enjoyment, learning goal orientation, and the technology acceptance model. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies 59 (4): 431–449.

Pincione, G., and F. R. Teson. 2006. Rational Choice and Democratic Deliberation: A Theory of Discourse Failure. Cambridge University Press. Pinsker, R., and S. Li. 2008. Costs and benefits of XBRL adoption: Early evidence. Communications of the ACM, 51 (3): 161-166. _______, and P. R. Wheeler. 2009. Nonprofessional investors’ perceptions of the efficiency and

effectiveness of XBRL-enabled financial statement analysis and of firms providing XBRL-formatted information. International Journal of Disclosure and Governance 6 (3): 241-262.

Page 34: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

32

_______, T. Pitre, and R. Daigle. 2009. An investigation of nonprofessional investors’ qualitative materiality judgments incorporating SEC listed vs. non-listed events. Journal of Accounting and Public Policy 28 (5): 446-465.

Plumlee, R. D., and M. A. Plumlee. 2008. Assurance on XBRL for financial reporting.

Accounting Horizons 22 (3): 353-368. Rose, J. 2002. Behavioral decision aid research: Decision aid use and effects. In Research

Accounting as an Information Systems Discipline, edited by V. Arnold and S. Sutton, 111-133. Sarasota FL: American Accounting Association.

SAS. 2005. Handbook for SAS users. Securities and Exchange Commission (SEC). 2009. Interactive data to improve financial

reporting. Available at: http://www.sec.gov/rules/final/2009/33-9002.pdf. Trites, G. 1999. The Impact of Technology on Financial and Business Reporting. Canadian Institute of Chartered Accountants, Toronto, Canada. Troshani, I., and S. Rao. 2007. Drivers and inhibitors to XBRL adoption: A qualitative approach

to build a theory in under-researched areas. International Journal of E-Business Research 3 (4): 98-111.

Tuttle, B., M. Coller, and F. G. Burton. 1997. An examination of market

efficiency: Information order effects in a laboratory market. Accounting, Organizations, and Society 22 (1): 89-103.

Vasarhelyi, M., D. Chan, and J. P. Krahel. 2011. XBRL: Consequences to financial reporting,

data analysis, decision support, and others. Working paper, Rutgers University. Vessey, I. 1991. Cognitive fit: a theory-based analysis of the graphs versus tables literature.

Decision Sciences 22 (2): 219-241. _______, and D. Galletta. 1991. Cognitive fit: an empirical study of information acquisition.

Information Systems Research 2 (1): 63-84. _______. 2006. The theory of cognitive fit: One aspect of a general theory of problem solving?

In Human-computer Interaction and Management Information Systems: Foundations, Advances in Management Information Systems Series, edited by P. Zhang and D. Galletta. Armonk, NY: M.E. Sharpe.

Wanous, J., A. Reichers, and M. Hudy. 1997. Overall job satisfaction: How good are single-item measures? Journal of Applied Psychology 82 (2): 247-252.

Page 35: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

33

Wheeler, P. R., and D. R. Jones. 2003. The effects of exclusive user choice of decision aid features on decision making. Journal of Information Systems 17 (1): 63-83.

_______, and V. Arunachalam. 2008. The effects of decision aid design on the information

search strategies and confirmation bias of tax professionals. Behavioral Research in Accounting 20 (1): 131-145.

Whitecotton, S., and S. Butler. 1998. Influencing decision aid reliance through involvement in information choice. Behavioral Research in Accounting 10 (Supplement): 182-200. XBRL International Standards Board (XSB). 2010. Preserve. Promote. Particpate. Moving

XBRL forward. Strategic initiative issued October. Available at: http://www.xbrl.org/P3%20docs/XBRL2010Initiatives.pdf

Page 36: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

34

TABLE 1 Participant Demographics

Demographics Frequencies Mean or % (Std. Dev.)

Age 30.06 (8.13) Years of business work experience 4.68 (7.61) Gender Male = 22 41.5% Female = 31 58.5% Bought or sold individual company’s No = 37 69.8% common stock or mutual fund Yes = 16 30.2% Bought or sold individual company’s common stock No = 46 86.8% or mutual fund as part of class exercise Yes = 7 13.2% Plan to invest in individual company’s common stock No = 18 34.0% in the next five years Yes = 35 66.0% Number of times evaluating a company’s performance 5.53 by analyzing its financial statements (16.94) Used an investment model to buy a stock/mutual fund No = 48 90.6% Yes = 5 9.4% Experience level:a Working with spreadsheets

7.21

(1.92) Working with XBRL-enabled technology

3.38

(2.90) Working with portable document files

6.26

(2.46) Difficulty of financial analysis taskb

5.08

(1.88) Rate the realism of this taskc

6.81

(1.68) a Participants were asked to rate their experience on an 11 point scale where 0 = novice and 10 = expert. b Participants were asked to rate the difficulty on an 11 point scale where 0 = extremely simple and 10 = extremely complex c Participants were asked to rate the realism of the task on an 11 point scale where 0 = unrealistic and 10 = very realistic

Page 37: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

35

TABLE 2 Pearson Correlations for All Hypothesized Variables

TECHNOLOGYCHOICE

(DV)

PERCEIVED EASE OF

USE PERCEIVED

USEFULNESS

PRIOR EXPERIENCE

WITH TECHNOLOGY

TAKES LESS TIME

TECHNOLOGY CHOICE (DV)

1.00

PERCEIVED EASE OF USE

0.16 1.00

PERCEIVED USEFULNESS

0.18 0.48 *** 1.00

PRIOR EXPERIENCE

WITH TECHNOLOGY

-0.48**** 0.05 -0.29 ** 1.00

TAKES LESS TIME

0.26 ** 0.38 *** 0.56 *** -0.23 ** 1.00

* one tailed p-value < 0.10 ** one-tailed p-value < 0.05 *** one-tailed p-value < 0.01

Page 38: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

36

TABLE 3 Results of Kruskal-Wallis Testing for Technology Choice Factors

Variable Choice N Mean Rank χ2 p-value

PERCEIVED USEFULNESS Excel XBRL

18 35

22.83 29.14

2.05 0.15

PERCEIVED EASE OF USE Excel XBRL

18 35

23.92 28.59

1.11 0.29

PRIOR EXPERIENCE Excel XBRL

18 35

37.81 21.44

13.51

<0.001

TAKES LESS TIME Excel XBRL

18 35

21.33 29.91

3.80 <0.05

Page 39: XBRL-Enabled Excel or PDF the Effects of Exclusive Technology

37

FIGURE 1 Experimental Procedure

Introduction

Train XBRL

Train Excel

Train PDF

Part I

Make TechnologyChoice

Perform Task

Part II Part III Complete Task Questions


Recommended