+ All Categories
Home > Documents > fMRI of the rod scotoma elucidates cortical rod pathways and implications for lesion measurements

fMRI of the rod scotoma elucidates cortical rod pathways and implications for lesion measurements

Date post: 16-May-2023
Category:
Upload: uci
View: 0 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
14
fMRI of the rod scotoma elucidates cortical rod pathways and implications for lesion measurements Brian Barton 1 and Alyssa A. Brewer Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of California, Irvine, CA 92697 Edited by Brian A. Wandell, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, and approved March 16, 2015 (received for review December 10, 2014) Are silencing, ectopic shifts, and receptive field (RF) scaling in cortical scotoma projection zones (SPZs) the result of long-term reorganization (plasticity) or short-term adaptation? Electrophys- iological studies of SPZs after retinal lesions in animal models remain controversial, because they are unable to conclusively answer this question because of limitations of the methodology. Here, we used functional MRI (fMRI) visual field mapping through population RF (pRF) modeling with moving bar stimuli under photopic and scotopic conditions to measure the effects of the rod scotoma in human early visual cortex. As a naturally occurring central scotoma, it has a large cortical representation, is free of traumatic lesion complications, is completely reversible, and has not reorganized under normal conditions (but can as seen in rod monochromats). We found that the pRFs overlapping the SPZ in V1, V2, V3, hV4, and VO-1 generally (i ) reduced their blood oxygen level-dependent signal coherence and (ii ) shifted their pRFs more eccentric but (iii ) scaled their pRF sizes in variable ways. Thus, silencing, ectopic shifts, and pRF scaling in SPZs are not unique identifiers of cortical reorganization; rather, they can be the expected result of short-term adaptation. However, are there differences between rod and cone signals in V1, V2, V3, hV4, and VO-1? We did not find differences for all five maps in more peripheral eccen- tricities outside of rod scotoma influence in coherence, eccentricity representation, or pRF size. Thus, rod and cone signals seem to be processed similarly in cortex. functional MRI | scotopic | plasticity | adaptation | population receptive fields A pressing question in visual neuroscience is, To what extent can adult human visual cortex reorganize after the removal of visual input?This question can be studied through the effects of retinal lesions (causing scotomas), in which input from the retina has been removed, but cortical representations of the scotoma projection zone (SPZ) remain intact. Accordingly, em- phasis must be placed on teasing apart effects of scotomas that relate to short-term cortical adaptation from those of long-term cortical plasticity (1) (terminology review is in ref. 2). Here, we investigate this question in human cortex by using functional MRI (fMRI) to measure the immediate cortical SPZ responses in the unique paradigm of the naturally occurring rod scotoma. The photoreceptors in humans can be divided into two classes: cones, which are primarily responsible for vision under high-luminance (photopic) conditions, and rods, which are primarily responsible for vision under low-luminance (scotopic) conditions when the cones are inactive. The cones are an order of magnitude more highly concentrated in the fovea relative to the periphery, where they inform our most detailed visual experience (3). In contrast, the greatest concentrations of rods are more than 10° eccentric from fixation and become increasingly sparse toward fixation until they are completely absent. This roughly circular rod- free zone covers a radius of 0.60.8° of visual angle about the fixation point (diameter = 1.251.7°) (4, 5). Under scotopic conditions, a scotoma arises from these foveal, rod-free zones, because no photoreceptors are stimulated within these regions (6, 7). Perceptual and fMRI estimates of the rod scotoma range from 1° to 2° of visual angle in radius because of the rod-sparse region surrounding the foveola and individual variability (69). The properties of the rod scotoma make it an excellent can- didate for studying the removal of visual input. First, the scotoma exists in all normal human subjects under scotopic conditions (5, 7). Second, the scotoma is located in the central fovea, which has large swaths of early visual cortex devoted to its analysis (1012). Third, the scotoma arises because of the central foveas complete lack and surrounding paucity of rod photoreceptors, allowing for a very close comparison with retinal lesions in animal models (5). Fourth, there is indirect evidence that the rod contributions to cortical activity are very similar to those of the cones, allowing for comparisons of changes in the properties of the cortical neurons overlapping the scotomas arising from either scotopic conditions or direct retinal lesions (6, 7, 13, 14). Fifth, the rod scotoma is completely reversible on return to photopic condi- tions, allowing for the measurement of ectopic cortical responses caused by short-term cortical adaptation without contamination from long-term reorganization, such as that seen in the relatively permanent developmental foveal scotomas of rod monochromats (7). Keys to the use of the rod scotoma in the evaluation of cortical plasticity are the questions, To what extent do the retinal dif- ferences between rod and cone photoreceptors influence cortical processing, and do any affects vary across cortical regions?Rods have larger receptive fields (RFs) than cones and greater connectivity density with ganglion cells (4, 5), but do these dif- ferences survive center-surround mutual inhibitory networks to be measurably different at the cortical level (15, 16)? To answer all of these questions, the cortical effects of the rod scotoma must be differentiated from any effects caused by differences between rod and cone input. To investigate the effects of the rod scotoma in human early visual cortex, we presently compared the retinotopic responses in Significance We use functional MRI to investigate the cortical effects on V1, V2, V3, hV4, and VO-1 when humanseyes have adapted to low-light vision. We show that populations of neurons with receptive fields interacting with the central rod scotoma are silenced because of lack of stimulation, shift their locations ectopically, and/or scale their sizes in some maps because of partial stimulation when the receptive fields overlap with the rod scotoma. These same effects have been cited as hallmarks of long-term reorganization, but our results show that these effects can be the result of the normal short-term adaptation of the visual system. In contrast, we observe no cortical dif- ferences between general rod and cone input other than rod scotoma effects. Author contributions: B.B. and A.A.B. designed research, performed research, analyzed data, and wrote the paper. The authors declare no conflict of interest. This article is a PNAS Direct Submission. 1 To whom correspondence should be addressed. Email: [email protected]. This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10. 1073/pnas.1423673112/-/DCSupplemental. www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1423673112 PNAS Early Edition | 1 of 6 NEUROSCIENCE
Transcript

fMRI of the rod scotoma elucidates cortical rodpathways and implications for lesion measurementsBrian Barton1 and Alyssa A. Brewer

Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of California, Irvine, CA 92697

Edited by Brian A. Wandell, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, and approved March 16, 2015 (received for review December 10, 2014)

Are silencing, ectopic shifts, and receptive field (RF) scaling incortical scotoma projection zones (SPZs) the result of long-termreorganization (plasticity) or short-term adaptation? Electrophys-iological studies of SPZs after retinal lesions in animal modelsremain controversial, because they are unable to conclusivelyanswer this question because of limitations of the methodology.Here, we used functional MRI (fMRI) visual field mapping throughpopulation RF (pRF) modeling with moving bar stimuli underphotopic and scotopic conditions to measure the effects of the rodscotoma in human early visual cortex. As a naturally occurringcentral scotoma, it has a large cortical representation, is free oftraumatic lesion complications, is completely reversible, and hasnot reorganized under normal conditions (but can as seen in rodmonochromats). We found that the pRFs overlapping the SPZ inV1, V2, V3, hV4, and VO-1 generally (i) reduced their blood oxygenlevel-dependent signal coherence and (ii) shifted their pRFs moreeccentric but (iii) scaled their pRF sizes in variable ways. Thus,silencing, ectopic shifts, and pRF scaling in SPZs are not uniqueidentifiers of cortical reorganization; rather, they can be the expectedresult of short-term adaptation. However, are there differencesbetween rod and cone signals in V1, V2, V3, hV4, and VO-1? Wedid not find differences for all five maps in more peripheral eccen-tricities outside of rod scotoma influence in coherence, eccentricityrepresentation, or pRF size. Thus, rod and cone signals seem to beprocessed similarly in cortex.

functional MRI | scotopic | plasticity | adaptation |population receptive fields

Apressing question in visual neuroscience is, “To what extentcan adult human visual cortex reorganize after the removal

of visual input?” This question can be studied through the effectsof retinal lesions (causing scotomas), in which input from theretina has been removed, but cortical representations of thescotoma projection zone (SPZ) remain intact. Accordingly, em-phasis must be placed on teasing apart effects of scotomas thatrelate to short-term cortical adaptation from those of long-termcortical plasticity (1) (terminology review is in ref. 2). Here, weinvestigate this question in human cortex by using functionalMRI (fMRI) to measure the immediate cortical SPZ responsesin the unique paradigm of the naturally occurring rod scotoma.The photoreceptors in humans can be divided into two

classes: cones, which are primarily responsible for vision underhigh-luminance (photopic) conditions, and rods, which are primarilyresponsible for vision under low-luminance (scotopic) conditionswhen the cones are inactive. The cones are an order of magnitudemore highly concentrated in the fovea relative to the periphery,where they inform our most detailed visual experience (3). Incontrast, the greatest concentrations of rods are more than ∼10°eccentric from fixation and become increasingly sparse towardfixation until they are completely absent. This roughly circular rod-free zone covers a radius of ∼0.6–0.8° of visual angle about thefixation point (diameter = ∼1.25–1.7°) (4, 5). Under scotopicconditions, a scotoma arises from these foveal, rod-free zones,because no photoreceptors are stimulated within these regions (6,7). Perceptual and fMRI estimates of the rod scotoma range from

∼1° to 2° of visual angle in radius because of the rod-sparse regionsurrounding the foveola and individual variability (6–9).The properties of the rod scotoma make it an excellent can-

didate for studying the removal of visual input. First, the scotomaexists in all normal human subjects under scotopic conditions (5,7). Second, the scotoma is located in the central fovea, which haslarge swaths of early visual cortex devoted to its analysis (10–12).Third, the scotoma arises because of the central fovea’s completelack and surrounding paucity of rod photoreceptors, allowing fora very close comparison with retinal lesions in animal models (5).Fourth, there is indirect evidence that the rod contributions tocortical activity are very similar to those of the cones, allowingfor comparisons of changes in the properties of the corticalneurons overlapping the scotomas arising from either scotopicconditions or direct retinal lesions (6, 7, 13, 14). Fifth, the rodscotoma is completely reversible on return to photopic condi-tions, allowing for the measurement of ectopic cortical responsescaused by short-term cortical adaptation without contaminationfrom long-term reorganization, such as that seen in the relativelypermanent developmental foveal scotomas of rod monochromats (7).Keys to the use of the rod scotoma in the evaluation of cortical

plasticity are the questions, “To what extent do the retinal dif-ferences between rod and cone photoreceptors influence corticalprocessing, and do any affects vary across cortical regions?”Rods have larger receptive fields (RFs) than cones and greaterconnectivity density with ganglion cells (4, 5), but do these dif-ferences survive center-surround mutual inhibitory networks tobe measurably different at the cortical level (15, 16)? To answerall of these questions, the cortical effects of the rod scotomamust be differentiated from any effects caused by differencesbetween rod and cone input.To investigate the effects of the rod scotoma in human early

visual cortex, we presently compared the retinotopic responses in

Significance

We use functional MRI to investigate the cortical effects on V1,V2, V3, hV4, and VO-1 when humans’ eyes have adapted tolow-light vision. We show that populations of neurons withreceptive fields interacting with the central rod scotoma aresilenced because of lack of stimulation, shift their locationsectopically, and/or scale their sizes in some maps because ofpartial stimulation when the receptive fields overlap with therod scotoma. These same effects have been cited as hallmarksof long-term reorganization, but our results show that theseeffects can be the result of the normal short-term adaptationof the visual system. In contrast, we observe no cortical dif-ferences between general rod and cone input other than rodscotoma effects.

Author contributions: B.B. and A.A.B. designed research, performed research, analyzeddata, and wrote the paper.

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.1To whom correspondence should be addressed. Email: [email protected].

This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1423673112/-/DCSupplemental.

www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1423673112 PNAS Early Edition | 1 of 6

NEU

ROSC

IENCE

early visual field maps V1, V2, V3, hV4, and VO-1 betweenphotopic and scotopic conditions in normal adults. We expectthree types of short-term adaptive responses from neurons in theSPZ of any scotoma in visual space. First, neurons with RFscompletely eclipsed by the SPZ should be silenced, resulting in areduction of neural activity to the spontaneous firing rate (Fig.1A). At this population-level fMRI measurement, this effect willbe reflected by a reduction in coherence. Second, neurons withRFs partially eclipsed by the SPZ should have an apparent ec-topic shift of their preferred centers, because they will continueto respond to the remaining (now decreased) visual space. Suchectopic shifts occur whether the preferred center of that neuronis within the SPZ (Fig. 1B) or adjacent (Fig. 1C). Third, it isexpected that neurons with RFs partially eclipsed by the SPZmay show a scaling of their RF sizes, although whether theseincrease or decrease in size is difficult to predict. Such neurons’new RF spans will necessarily be reduced by the overlap with theSPZ, but their RF sizes may also be increased because of changesin feedback activity or a reduction in lateral inhibitory connec-tions to nearby neurons that have also been silenced or shifted toectopic locations by the scotoma (1, 2, 17–20). The combinationof these effects could also lead to no observable change at thislevel of measurement.Each of these predicted scotoma effects must be distinguished

from differences between photopic and scotopic conditions un-related to the rod scotoma. The RFs of neurons in more pe-ripheral eccentricities do not overlap with the rod scotoma, butthey perform the same computations as their more centralcounterparts. As a result, they measure the effect of rod vs. coneinputs independent of the rod scotoma, acting as an ideal controlwith which the effects of the rod scotoma can be contrasted.First, because of the difference in luminance that defines photopicand scotopic conditions, we predict a reduction in coherence.Second, we predict no change in the locations of cortical RFs.Third, we test whether there is a change in RF size at the pop-ulation level. The RFs of rods are larger than cones, and theretinal ganglion cells receive a greater number of inputs from rods

than cones (4, 5), both of which suggest that we may observe largercortical RFs. However, it is possible that, by the time that thesignals reach cortex, center-surround mutual inhibition circuitrymay counter any such increase in RF size, because the largerretinal RFs contribute to both the centers and surrounds of sub-cortical and cortical RFs (4, 5, 8, 13–16). Furthermore, any pop-ulation of RFs in a cortical location, such as a voxel, will have adegree of dispersion of their preferred centers, which leads to alarger measured RF for the population as a whole relative to in-dividual constituent RFs. As such, any change in RF size underscotopic relative to photopic conditions that survives center-sur-round mutual inhibitory networks may be indistinguishable fromRF dispersion at the population level (17, 21, 22). Any differencesin these measurements of coherence, preferred center, and size ofpopulations of RFs caused by the rod scotoma in the central ec-centricities must extend beyond any differences observed in moreperipheral eccentricities caused by differences between photopicand scotopic conditions.

ResultsTo compare cortical activity in early visual field maps betweenphotopic (luminance = 140 cd/m2) and scotopic (luminance =0.003 cd/m2) conditions, we collected fMRI data in four subjectsusing moving bar stimuli (Fig. 2F) after the subjects adapted toeach luminance condition (SI Materials and Methods). We usedpopulation RF (pRF) modeling to estimate the V1, V2, V3, hV4,and VO-1 maps and pRFs (22). A pRF for a particular voxelreflects the central tendency of the sizes (spreads) and centers invisual space preferentially activated by the RFs of the populationof neurons within that voxel that are activated by a particularstimulus. An example of the similarity of pRF model fits underphotopic and scotopic conditions is presented in Fig. S1. Foranalysis of the measurements of visual field map activity, shifts ofpRF centers, and scaling of pRF sizes, we divided up the ec-centricity representation in each map in each hemisphere of eachsubject into 10 regions of interest (ROIs) spanning 1° of visualangle along the eccentricity gradient from 0° to 10° centered onevery 0.5°. Each measurement was drawn from these 10 eccentricityband ROIs for each subject, averaged between hemispheres foreach subject, and then, analyzed across subjects between conditions.We defined the central ROI in each early visual field maps as

the maximum region with pRF sizes that are expected to overlapthe rod scotoma as measured in the photopic condition (Fig.1D). For example, voxels in V1 with a preferred center of 2.5° ofvisual angle are estimated by pRF measurements to span ∼1° ofvisual angle under photopic conditions and thus, would beexpected to span visual space approximately from 1.5° to 3.5°,which overlaps the visual span of the rod scotoma. Similarly,voxels in hV4 with a preferred center of 4.5° of visual angle areestimated by photopic pRF measurements to span ∼3.5°; thesevoxels would, thus, be expected to respond to visual space over aregion approximately from 1° to 8°, again partially overlappingthe rod scotoma. Therefore, in the following sections, we ex-amine differences between photopic and scotopic conditions inthe photopically defined eccentricity representations affected bythe rod scotoma (0–3° for V1 and V2, 0–4° for V3, 0–5° for hV4,and 0–6° for VO-1) and contrast those results with repre-sentations not affected by the rod scotoma (3–10° for V1 and V2,4–10° in V3, 5–10° in hV4, and 6–10° in VO-1).What follows are two multivariate ANOVA comparisons

(central and peripheral eccentricities) between photopic andscotopic conditions per measurement type (coherence, preferredeccentricity, and pRF size) (23). Data from the central eccen-tricities of each map were used to evaluate the effects of the rodscotoma on the neural activity within the SPZ, whereas datafrom the more peripheral eccentricities were used to evaluatedifferences between cone and rod inputs. Each measurement wasevaluated across subjects to assess group-level results, which is

Fig. 1. Schematic of the predicted effects of the rod scotoma. (A–C) Blackdisks and black circles around them indicate the preferred center and spreadof a neuron’s RF, respectively. Each row, thus, represents neurons withpreferred centers at one specific eccentricity. (Lower) The gray shaded re-gions indicate the SPZ of the rod scotoma under scotopic conditions. Blackarrows indicate the expected direction of the measured shift of RF centerscaused by interaction of the rod scotoma with a neuron’s RF under scotopicrelative to photopic conditions. (A) Neurons with RFs completely eclipsed bythe SPZ. (B) Neurons with RFs partially eclipsed by the SPZ and centers withinthe SPZ. (C) Neurons with RFs partially eclipsed by the SPZ and centersoutside the SPZ. (D) pRF interactions with the rod scotoma. This graph is anaccurately scaled visual representation of the normal pRF sizes (measuredunder photopic conditions) for each visual field map (degrees of visual anglecorresponding to the sizes seen here are shown in Fig. S6). Each circle is anaccurately scaled visual representation of the average size of pRFs for theeccentricities indicated below it in the map indicated on the left. The ec-centricities labeled at the bottom are the centers of the 1° bins used for 10eccentricity-band ROIs measured for each visual field map. Filled circlesrepresent pRFs in eccentricities where the pRF is both large enough and closeenough to the rod scotoma to expect interactions between them. Opencircles represent pRFs at eccentricities outside the expected influence of therod scotoma (see also Fig. S3).

2 of 6 | www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1423673112 Barton and Brewer

only possible for such detailed measurements through the pres-ently used functional (not anatomical) localization (24, 25).

Scotopic and Photopic Visual Field Map Measurements. Typical pRFmeasurements of the eccentricity and polar angle representa-tions in V1, V2, V3, hV4, and VO-1 under photopic and scotopicconditions are presented in Fig. 2 for the left hemisphere of onesubject (Fig. S2). Note the loss of blood oxygen level-dependentresponse in the central foveal representation (darkest red voxelsdrop out of the image in Fig. 2B) in the eccentricity represen-tation of V1 under scotopic conditions (Fig. 2B and Fig. S2 B, D,and F), consistent with previous findings (6, 7). Interestingly, V2,V3, hV4, and VO-1 did not show a similar loss of signal butrather, a peripheral shift in their central eccentricity represen-tations between photopic and scotopic conditions (red/orangevoxels shift to orange/yellow/green in Fig. 2B and Fig. S2 B, D,and F). This eccentricity shift represented in the color overlayshere is further quantified in graphical form in Fig. 3B. It is likelythat this shift was produced as neurons with small RFs within thisregion in each visual field map that did not overlap the rod sco-toma edge were silenced, whereas the neurons with larger RFsoverlapping the scotoma border are measured as effectively rep-resenting a more peripheral position (Fig. S3B) (17). The movingbar stimulus at this size (Fig. 2F) typically does not produce clearmeasurements of the polar angle representation within the verycentral fovea (hence, cyan color in the central photopic polar

angle map of V1 in Fig. 2C and Fig. S2 A, C, and E) but contrastswith the loss of contralateral responses in this region under sco-topic conditions (Fig. 2D and Fig. S2 B, D, and F). Outside of thisregion, the polar angle representations in all visual field mapsremained largely unchanged between the two conditions. Becausedifferences in eye movements between conditions could contributeto problems in measuring visual field maps, we confirmed that nosignificant differences in fixation stability existed between the twoconditions (26) (SI Materials and Methods, Fig. S4, and Table S1).

Neural Activity Is Reduced Within the SPZ.Coherence was measuredfor voxels in each visual field map across the entire stimulatedvisual field and compared between scotopic and photopic con-ditions to assess changes in blood oxygen level-dependent signalcaused by the differences in luminance (Fig. 3A, SI Materials andMethods, and Fig. S5). Across all five maps, photopic coherencewas not statistically significantly greater than scotopic coherencein the more peripheral eccentricities (ps = 0.078–0.897), whichindicates that responses in early visual areas are generally robustunder scotopic conditions, despite the drastic drop in luminanceand the activation of an entirely different class of photoreceptorsbetween photopic and scotopic conditions (Figs. S4A and S5 andTable S2). Within the central eccentricities of each map, wherepRFs interact with the rod scotoma, we observed significantdecreases in the coherence of V1 (P = 0.016), V2 (P = 0.044),and V3 (P = 0.013) but not hV4 (P = 0.624) or VO-1 (P = 0.465)(Fig. 3A, Fig. S5, and Table S2).These results indicate that there is a specific drop in neural

activity in these visual field maps caused by the silencing of neu-rons with RFs partially or completely eclipsed by the rod scotoma(Fig. S3). The pattern of results across visual field maps is con-sistent with the pRF sizes for each of the maps (Fig. 1D), such thatmaps with larger pRFs—hV4 and VO-1, which have proportion-ally less surface area eclipsed by the rod scotoma—are not

Fig. 2. Visual field maps in photopic and scotopic conditions. (A–D) Pseu-docolor overlays on a flattened representation of occipital cortex from theleft hemisphere of one subject (S2) represent the position in visual space thatproduces the strongest response at that cortical location. (A and B) Eccen-tricity representations. Color legend represents the visual field from 0° to 10°radius of visual angle. (C and D) Polar angle representations. Color legendrepresents the contralateral hemifield. (A and C) Photopic measurements.(B and D) Scotopic measurements. Boundaries of visual field maps are depictedwith dotted (polar angle boundaries between maps of interest) and solid (ec-centricity boundaries and edge of measurement) black lines. Coherence ≥ 0.20.(Scale bar: 1 cm along the flattened cortical surface.) (E, Upper) Anatomicalorientation legend. (E, Lower) Inflated 3D representation of a medial view ofthe left hemisphere of subject 2. Inset indicates the region near the calcarinesulcus of the occipital lobe, where the maps were measured. (F) Movingbar stimulus for visual field map and pRF measurements comprised a set ofcontrast-reversing checkerboard patterns at eccentricities from 0° to 11° radius.One frame is shown for the bar stimulus sequence. Four bar orientations(0°, 45°, 90°, and 135° from vertical) with two motion directions orthogonal toeach orientation were used, producing eight different bar configurations.Additional examples are in Fig. S2.

Fig. 3. Coherence, eccentricity shifts, and pRF size changes in central andperipheral eccentricities. (A) Coherence differences. (B) Ectopic eccentricityshifts. Positive numbers indicate shifts of pRF centers outward from the rodscotoma, and negative numbers indicate shifts of pRF centers into the rodscotoma. (C) Scotopic pRF size percentage changes. Positive values indicatelarger pRF sizes under scotopic conditions, whereas negative values indicatesmaller pRF sizes under scotopic conditions. All data are plotted as a functionof the photopic eccentricity in degrees of visual angle. The legend indicatesline shading and marker shape for each map. Error bars represent SEMs(Figs. S5–S7).

Barton and Brewer PNAS Early Edition | 3 of 6

NEU

ROSC

IENCE

significantly silenced within the central representation. Con-versely, maps with smaller pRF sizes—V1, V2, and V3, whichare proportionally more eclipsed by the rod scotoma—do un-dergo significant silencing of neural activity within the centralrepresentation (2, 27).

Voxels with pRFs Overlapping the Rod Scotoma Show an Ectopic Shiftin Their pRF Centers. Preferred eccentricity was measured forvoxels in each visual field map across the entire stimulated visualfield and compared between scotopic and photopic conditions toassess changes caused by the differences in luminance (Fig. 3Band Fig. S6). Across the more peripheral eccentricities of all fivemaps, there was no significant peripheral shift in pRF locationsfor scotopic relative to photopic conditions (ps = 0.091–0.740)(Fig. 3B, Fig. S6, and Table S2). However, there was a significantshift peripherally from the rod scotoma in scotopic relative tophotopic conditions for the central eccentricities of all five maps(ps = 0.023–0.049) (Fig. 3B, Fig. S6, and Table S2).These findings pose a significant problem for fMRI and

electrophysiological studies of cortical responses to scotomasreporting ectopic responses from a population of neurons in theSPZ as evidence of reorganization without taking into accounteffects of short-term adaptation (28–33). Here, we achieved thesame results in the SPZ of the rod scotoma with short-term ex-posure to scotopic conditions. In fMRI measurements, eachvoxel summarizes the summed activity of hundreds of thousandsof neurons, but if a substantial number of those neurons is si-lenced, because their RF is over the scotoma, the summed RFnow only draws from the more active, peripheral individual RFsand shifts more eccentric from the scotoma, which we see here.Similarly, in electrophysiological studies of single neurons, theseshort-term changes in measurements may arise because of asimilar effect at the level of the retinal ganglion cells; the RFsof the neurons in this case each may be drawing from severalganglion cells, some of which are silenced within the scotoma,whereas the more peripheral ganglion cells remain active. Studiesof long-term cortical plasticity using any measurement must showthat any ectopic responses caused by long-term reorganization areabove and beyond the effects of such short-term cortical adapta-tion (2, 17). Note that fMRI measurements of ectopic responsesfrom long-term reorganization have been successfully shown inthe rod scotoma SPZs in rod monochromats (which have acondition that produces congenital, bilateral foveal lesions in therod-free zone) (7) by comparing these ectopic responses in therod monochromat subjects with rod scotoma SPZ measurementsin control subjects.

Voxels with pRFs Overlapping the SPZ May Scale pRF Sizes. pRF sizeswere measured for voxels in each visual field map across theentire stimulated visual field and compared between scotopicand photopic conditions to assess changes caused by the differ-ences in luminance (Fig. 3C and Fig. S7). The scaling of pRFsizes was evaluated using a measure of the percentage change inpRF sizes between luminance conditions: pRF size percentagechange = (scotopic pRF size/photopic pRF size − 1) × 100.Positive values would indicate a larger pRF size under scotopicconditions, whereas negative values would indicate smaller pRFsizes under scotopic conditions. Across the more peripheral ec-centricities of all five maps, there was no significant pRF sizescaling in scotopic relative to photopic conditions (ps = 0.200–0.628) (Fig. 3C, Fig. S7, and Table S2). There also was not a sig-nificant difference in pRF size percentage change between phot-opic and scotopic conditions for V2, V3, or hV4 (ps = 0.131–0.436).However, there was a marginally significant pRF size percentagedecrease in the central eccentricities for V1 (P = 0.062) and asignificant increase for VO-1 (P = 0.015) (Fig. 3C, Fig. S7, andTable S2).

These results indicate that scaling is quite variable among mapsbecause of the rod scotoma; decreasing, not changing, and in-creasing pRF sizes were all observed. It is possible that thesechanges reflect differences in attentional modulation from higher-order visual field maps or perhaps, differences in the properties ofthe individual maps, such as differences in initial pRF sizes orlateral connectivity (2, 17, 18, 34). For the more peripheral ec-centricities, these results indicate that there is no observable cor-tical difference at this measurement level in pRF size between therod and cone processing pathways.

DiscussionIn summary, the central eccentricities of these visual field maps,with pRFs overlapping the SPZ, generally (i) reduced their co-herence because of silenced neurons, (ii) shifted their pRFcenters more eccentric from the rod scotoma, and (iii) hadvariable results regarding scaling of their pRF sizes (increase,decrease, and no change). Each of these measurements was in-dependent of long-term plasticity, which has particularly im-portant implications for the interpretation of studies of corticalreorganization. Although several electrophysiological and fMRIstudies propose long-term cortical reorganization as the primarymechanism for silencing, ectopic shifts, and pRF scaling withinthe SPZ (29, 31–33, 35), we have acquired a similar pRF-levelmeasurement and shown that these responses can arise duringshort-term cortical adaptation in the representation of pRFs withsome overlap with the SPZ.In contrast, the more peripheral eccentricities of these visual

field maps, with pRFs independent of the rod scotoma, (i) hadno statistically significant reductions in coherence, (ii) did notshift their pRF centers, and (iii) did not scale their pRF sizes.Although there was no significant reduction in coherence, theretended to be a nonsignificant drop in coherence. Crucially, thedrop in coherence observed in the central eccentricities is largerthan in the midperiphery. In general, these results act as an idealwithin-subject, within-map control, differentiating the effects ofthe rod scotoma from the luminance drop between photopic andscotopic conditions. Furthermore, these results indicate thatretinal differences between rod and cone photoreceptors do nottranslate to measurable differences at the cortical level of thesefive visual field maps.

Comparisons with Studies on the Cortical Effects of Other RetinalScotomas. Keys to understanding cortical responses to scotomasare the following questions. Are silencing, ectopic shifts, and RFscaling in cortical SPZs the result of short-term adaptation andthus, the expected immediate response of the visual system to aremoval of visual input? Alternatively, do such responses pri-marily arise over a longer period after more extensive corticalreorganization? Scotomas caused by trauma or disease arechallenging for human SPZ studies, because they tend to bepermanent, monocular, variable in retinal thicknesses, and dif-ficult to compare across patients because of variability in retinalor cortical location and time of onset. The blind spots, althoughthey are omnipresent, naturally occurring scotomas, are locatedin the midperiphery, where there is less cortex devoted to visualanalysis, and the RFs are larger relative to the fovea (4, 5, 10).Not surprisingly, fMRI studies of the blind spot can only localizea small area of V1 corresponding to the blind spot, which doesnot make it ideally suited for these questions of ectopic re-sponses and plasticity (36, 37). Artificial, reversible scotomascaused by stabilization of the stimulus on the retina are transientand most effective in the periphery (38), making them difficult tomeasure in early visual areas with fMRI because of the com-paratively slow temporal resolution of fMRI and the relativelysmaller amount of cortex devoted to peripheral processing (10,11, 39, 40).

4 of 6 | www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1423673112 Barton and Brewer

As a result, many researchers have focused on studying long-term reorganization in early visual cortex in cat and monkey inresponse to induced binocular retinal lesions, but even thesestudies have very controversial results (2, 41). Some groups usingelectrophysiology have reported silencing, ectopic shifts, andpRF scaling from V1 neurons within the SPZ that they use asevidence of cortical reorganization weeks to months after theretinae were lesioned (28–33). However, because these studiestypically measure only immediate postlesion and long-term (weeksto months) time points, they cannot differentiate responses in theSPZ caused by short-term adaptation from those arising from long-term reorganization (Fig. S3). The retinal tissue surrounding theexperimentally lesioned site takes up to 2 wk to recover normalfunction after initial swelling from such lesion-inducing proceduresas photocoagulation, which prevents accurate measurements im-mediately after the retinal lesion (27, 28, 42). Furthermore, mea-surements of short-term adaptation in these cases are inherentlyconfounded by the effects of this retinal stunning that surrounds thetrue retinal lesion.In contrast, other groups using electrophysiology and fMRI

report no evidence of ectopic responses in the macaque V1 SPZafter weeks of recovery and therefore, no cortical reorganization(2, 27). It is important to note that none of these studies canmeasure the same neuron at multiple time points, but rather,they must sample from active neurons in similar locations,resulting in potential sampling biases that further complicatetheir interpretations (2, 27). Similar conflicting measurementshave been seen in human patients with bilateral foveal lesionsfrom age-related macular degeneration, with some fMRI studiesclaiming extensive recovery within the V1 SPZ, whereas again,others showed no evidence for reorganization (7, 18, 35, 43–45).Thus, the predicted and measured effects on (p)RFs caused

by short-term adaptation and long-term reorganization are verydifficult to differentiate (Fig. S3). To avoid a potential over-estimation of the extent of long-term cortical reorganization andrecovery within the SPZ, measurements must be able to determinethat long-term reorganization has occurred that is greater thanboth what can be attributed to short-term adaptation, as describedhere, and what can be attributed to the recovery of the stunnedretinal tissue. Often, one could argue that silencing, shifting, andscaling of (p)RFs in response to a scotoma are short-term adap-tations at work but have simply been mistaken for long-termcortical reorganization in many cases. Our goal here was to usemeasurements of the human rod SPZ to allow for detailed eval-uation of immediate human cortical responses to the reversibleremoval of visual input.Our data are largely consistent with and extend the recent

findings that central retinal lesions caused by age-related mac-ular degeneration and simulated lesions in the central (5° and7.5° radius) visual field in control subjects show ectopic pRFshifts, silencing, and scaling in V1 caused by cortical short-termadaptation rather than long-term reorganization (17, 18). Wenote that our data differ in our measurements of a small, mar-ginally significant decrease in pRF size in V1 caused by the rodscotoma, whereas these studies showed an increase in V1 pRFsize caused by age-related macular degeneration and artificialscotomas. One possibility for this difference is that our barstimulus, which spans the central visual field, may have elicited agreater perception of filling in than the expanding ring and ro-tating wedge stimuli used in the prior studies, leading to alter-ations in pRF dynamics (46). We have compared such differencesin perceptual filling in between these stimulus types in othermeasurements and do find greater perceptual filling in for the barstimulus (47). Another factor may be the differences in the sizes ofthe scotomas; the previously measured scotomas are much larger,on average, than the rod scotoma presently measured. The largerscotoma increases the average pRF size affected by the scotomabecause of the well-documented enlargement in pRF size from the

representation of central fixation to that of the periphery in visualfield maps (22). Interestingly, we do observe increases in pRF sizescaused by the rod scotoma for larger pRFs, which we measured inVO-1, consistent with these previous findings in V1 (Fig. 1D showsa model of pRF sizes by map and eccentricity). We do not believethat this difference in V1 pRF sizes arises from the differentphotoreceptors activated in each study (rods vs. cones). We expectthat any such differences in the rod vs. cone visual pathways wouldbe evident across our analysis of the peripheral eccentricities ofthe visual field (7). However, we see no differences betweenphotopic and scotopic conditions in the relatively more peripheraleccentricities of any of five visual field maps.

Rod Pathways in Cortex.Comparatively few studies have examinedthe contributions of the rod system to cortical activity. Ourmeasurements support the studies of scotopic psychophysics andretinal circuitry, which suggest that most, if not all, retinal gan-glion cell types—and thus, the cone pathways—contribute toscotopic vision (13, 14, 48). Outside of the region of interactionswith the rod scotoma, we do not measure any differences in vi-sual field map organization or pRF properties across our mea-surements out to 10° of visual angle between photopic andscotopic conditions, suggesting that these regions in these visualfield maps receive similar cone and rod inputs, at least for thislevel of processing.With measurements of cortical activation under scotopic vi-

sion, Hadjikhani and Tootell (6) also showed similar peripheralresponses between photopic and scotopic conditions in V1, V2,V3, and what they measured as V8 (analogous to our hV4 andVO-1) (39), comparable with our findings. However, Hadjikhaniand Tootell (6) observed a significant lack of activity in thecentral representations of their four maps, which is in contrast toour findings of shifts of the central representations to moreparafoveal regions in all five maps. We do observe significantdecreases of activity, which indicate that many neurons in pRFsoverlapping the rod scotoma have reduced activity or are si-lenced. These differences may have arisen from variations in (i)measurement methodology (traveling wave vs. pRF modeling),(ii) signal-to-noise ratios, and (iii) effects arising from the stim-ulus types, such as perceptual filling in. Our moving bar stimulus,for example, likely produced a greater effect of filling in, whichmight be reflected in top-down influences producing activity infoveal V2, V3, hV4, and VO-1 (19, 47). Along these lines, arecent study by Williams et al. (19) showed that feedback fromhigher cortical areas can produce differential effects in the foveavs. the periphery of early visual cortex. Specifically, object stimulipresented in the periphery produced responses in foveal reti-notopic cortex but not peripheral retinotopic cortex. It is possiblethat a similar feedback mechanism contributes to the coherencechanges, ectopic shifts, and pRF size changes measured herealong the scotoma border.Additionally, Hadjikhani and Tootell (6) observed no activa-

tion of the region of hV4 and VO-1 (V8) under scotopic con-ditions throughout their entire measured visual field, concludingthat those maps were cone-only color-processing maps. We ob-serve significant activation of both hV4 and VO-1 when onlyrods are active, indicating that, although these maps may be in-volved in color processing, they are not cone-exclusive. From ourdata, it seems that rod signals are passed through all maps in theearly stages of the visual system. There seems to be no differ-ences in any of our measurements of the peripheral eccentrici-ties, indicating that the maps handle rod signals very similarly tocone signals.Finally, we note that our measurements average across all

polar angles within each eccentricity band. Interestingly, there isgrowing evidence for perceptual and neural asymmetries be-tween the dorsal and ventral visual fields, with improved motion,global processing, and coordinate spatial judgments in the lower

Barton and Brewer PNAS Early Edition | 5 of 6

NEU

ROSC

IENCE

visual field and improved visual search, local processing, andcategorical judgments in the upper visual field, especially for theleft visual field (49–53). Such variations would not be apparent inour measurements based on eccentricity from fixation, whichgrouped the polar angles. Although such differences would beunlikely to have an effect on our results here, it may be of interestto future studies to investigate potential differences between thesequarter-field representations as well as between hemispheres.

ConclusionsThe use of the rod scotoma provides an excellent, reversible,accessible approach for investigating the short-term responses toscotomas as well as the cortical differences between rod and coneinputs, which we describe here. Claims of long-term cortical re-covery in response to retinal scotomas must take into accountthese rapid cortical adjustments as well as retinal recovery fromstunning postlesion and the resulting return to normal function atthe edge of the SPZ before being able to conclusively attributethese cortical responses to long-term reorganization. Additionally,

rod and cone contributions to V1, V2, V3, hV4, and VO-1 appearquite similar when unaffected by the rod scotoma in peripheraleccentricities, which indicates that cone and rod inputs are treatedvery similarly at the cortical level.

Materials and MethodsSubjects. Four subjects (two females) ages 24–36 years old participated in thisstudy. All subjects had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity. The ex-perimental protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board atUniversity of California, Irvine, and informed consent was obtained fromall subjects.

Experimental Design. Each subject underwent two fMRI scan sessions, whichinvolved collecting 1 T1-weighted anatomical volume, 2 T1-weighted in-plane anatomical scans, and 16 functional visual field mapping scans (movingbar stimulus) under both photopic (luminance = 140 cd/m2) and scotopic(luminance = 0.003 cd/m2) conditions (8 scans per condition). Our dataanalysis used pRF modeling to estimate the visual field map organizationand pRF sizes and centers (22) (SI Materials and Methods).

1. Dreher B, Burke W, Calford MB (2001) Cortical plasticity revealed by circumscribedretinal lesions or artificial scotomas. Prog Brain Res 134:217–246.

2. Wandell BA, Smirnakis SM (2009) Plasticity and stability of visual field maps in adultprimary visual cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci 10(12):873–884.

3. Roorda A, Williams DR (1999) The arrangement of the three cone classes in the livinghuman eye. Nature 397(6719):520–522.

4. Curcio CA, Allen KA (1990) Topography of ganglion cells in human retina. J CompNeurol 300(1):5–25.

5. Curcio CA, Sloan KR, Kalina RE, Hendrickson AE (1990) Human photoreceptor to-pography. J Comp Neurol 292(4):497–523.

6. Hadjikhani N, Tootell RB (2000) Projection of rods and cones within human visualcortex. Hum Brain Mapp 9(1):55–63.

7. Baseler HA, et al. (2002) Reorganization of human cortical maps caused by inheritedphotoreceptor abnormalities. Nat Neurosci 5(4):364–370.

8. Duffy KR, Hubel DH (2007) Receptive field properties of neurons in the primary visualcortex under photopic and scotopic lighting conditions. Vision Res 47(19):2569–2574.

9. Hubel DH, Howe PD, Duffy AM, Hernández A (2009) Scotopic foveal afterimages.Perception 38(2):313–316.

10. Dougherty RF, et al. (2003) Visual field representations and locations of visual areasV1/2/3 in human visual cortex. J Vis 3(10):586–598.

11. Wandell BA, Dumoulin SO, Brewer AA (2007) Visual field maps in human cortex.Neuron 56(2):366–383.

12. Schira MM, Tyler CW, Breakspear M, Spehar B (2009) The foveal confluence in humanvisual cortex. J Neurosci 29(28):9050–9058.

13. Field GD, et al. (2009) High-sensitivity rod photoreceptor input to the blue-yellowcolor opponent pathway in macaque retina. Nat Neurosci 12(9):1159–1164.

14. Stockman A, Candler T, Sharpe LT (2010) Human scotopic sensitivity is regulated post-receptorally by changing the speed of the scotopic response. J Vis 10(2):12.1–12.19.

15. Xing J, Heeger DJ (2000) Center-surround interactions in foveal and peripheral vision.Vision Res 40(22):3065–3072.

16. Xing J, Heeger DJ (2001) Measurement and modeling of center-surround suppressionand enhancement. Vision Res 41(5):571–583.

17. Haak KV, Cornelissen FW, Morland AB (2012) Population receptive field dynamics inhuman visual cortex. PLoS ONE 7(5):e37686.

18. Baseler HA, et al. (2011) Large-scale remapping of visual cortex is absent in adulthumans with macular degeneration. Nat Neurosci 14(5):649–655.

19. Williams MA, et al. (2008) Feedback of visual object information to foveal retinotopiccortex. Nat Neurosci 11(12):1439–1445.

20. Sirotin YB, Das A (2009) Anticipatory haemodynamic signals in sensory cortex notpredicted by local neuronal activity. Nature 457(7228):475–479.

21. Lehky SR, Sereno AB (2011) Population coding of visual space: Modeling. FrontComput Neurosci 4:155.

22. Dumoulin SO, Wandell BA (2008) Population receptive field estimates in human visualcortex. Neuroimage 39(2):647–660.

23. Brewer AA, Barton B (2014) Visual cortex in aging and Alzheimer’s disease: Changesin visual field maps and population receptive fields. Front Psychol 5:74.

24. Brewer AA, Barton B (2012) Visual field map organization in human visual cortex.Visual Cortex—Current Status and Perspectives, eds Molotchnikoff S, Rouat J (InTech,New York), pp 29–60.

25. Brewer AA, Barton B (2014) Developmental plasticity: FMRI investigations into humanvisual cortex. Advanced Brain Neuroimaging Topics in Health and Disease—Methodsand Applications, eds apageorgiou DT, Smirnakis SM, Christopoulos G (InTech, NewYork), pp 305–334.

26. Beauchamp MS (2003) Detection of eye movements from fMRI data. Magn ResonMed 49(2):376–380.

27. Smirnakis SM, et al. (2005) Lack of long-term cortical reorganization after macaqueretinal lesions. Nature 435(7040):300–307.

28. Giannikopoulos DV, Eysel UT (2006) Dynamics and specificity of cortical map re-organization after retinal lesions. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103(28):10805–10810.

29. Calford MB, et al. (2005) Neuroscience: Rewiring the adult brain. Nature 438(7065):E3.30. Darian-Smith C, Gilbert CD (1994) Axonal sprouting accompanies functional re-

organization in adult cat striate cortex. Nature 368(6473):737–740.31. Gilbert CD, Wiesel TN (1992) Receptive field dynamics in adult primary visual cortex.

Nature 356(6365):150–152.32. Heinen SJ, Skavenski AA (1991) Recovery of visual responses in foveal V1 neurons

following bilateral foveal lesions in adult monkey. Exp Brain Res 83(3):670–674.33. Kaas JH (1991) Plasticity of sensory and motor maps in adult mammals. Annu Rev

Neurosci 14:137–167.34. Ress D, Backus BT, Heeger DJ (2000) Activity in primary visual cortex predicts per-

formance in a visual detection task. Nat Neurosci 3(9):940–945.35. Baker CI, Peli E, Knouf N, Kanwisher NG (2005) Reorganization of visual processing in

macular degeneration. J Neurosci 25(3):614–618.36. Awater H, Kerlin JR, Evans KK, Tong F (2005) Cortical representation of space around

the blind spot. J Neurophysiol 94(5):3314–3324.37. Dilks DD, Baker CI, Liu Y, Kanwisher N (2009) “Referred visual sensations”: Rapid

perceptual elongation after visual cortical deprivation. J Neurosci 29(28):8960–8964.38. Ramachandran VS (1992) Filling in the blind spot. Nature 356(6365):115.39. Brewer AA, Liu J, Wade AR, Wandell BA (2005) Visual field maps and stimulus se-

lectivity in human ventral occipital cortex. Nat Neurosci 8(8):1102–1109.40. Wandell BA, Brewer AA, Dougherty RF (2005) Visual field map clusters in human

cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 360(1456):693–707.41. Hooks BM, Chen C (2007) Critical periods in the visual system: Changing views for a

model of experience-dependent plasticity. Neuron 56(2):312–326.42. DeAngelis GC, Anzai A, Ohzawa I, Freeman RD (1995) Receptive field structure in the

visual cortex: Does selective stimulation induce plasticity? Proc Natl Acad Sci USA92(21):9682–9686.

43. Liu T, et al. (2010) Incomplete cortical reorganization in macular degeneration. InvestOphthalmol Vis Sci 51(12):6826–6834.

44. Masuda Y, Dumoulin SO, Nakadomari S, Wandell BA (2008) V1 projection zone signals

in human macular degeneration depend on task, not stimulus. Cereb Cortex 18(11):2483–2493.

45. Sunness JS, Liu T, Yantis S (2004) Retinotopic mapping of the visual cortex usingfunctional magnetic resonance imaging in a patient with central scotomas fromatrophic macular degeneration. Ophthalmology 111(8):1595–1598.

46. Komatsu H (2006) The neural mechanisms of perceptual filling-in. Nat Rev Neurosci7(3):220–231.

47. Barton B, Brewer AA (2011) FMRI of the rod cotoma: Filling-in, rod pathway pro-jections, and insights into plasticity. Journal of Vision 11(15):9.

48. Buck SL, Knight RF, Bechtold J (2000) Opponent-color models and the influence of rod

signals on the loci of unique hues. Vision Res 40(24):3333–3344.49. Lee B, Kaneoke Y, Kakigi R, Sakai Y (2009) Human brain response to visual stimulus

between lower/upper visual fields and cerebral hemispheres. Int J Psychophysiol 74(2):81–87.

50. Thomas NA, Elias LJ (2011) Upper and lower visual field differences in perceptual

asymmetries. Brain Res 1387:108–115.51. Danckert J, Goodale MA (2001) Superior performance for visually guided pointing in

the lower visual field. Exp Brain Res 137(3-4):303–308.52. Skrandies W (1987) The upper and lower visual field of man: Electrophysiological and

functional differences. Prog Sens Physiol 8:1–93.53. Previc FH (1990) Functional specialization in the lower and upper visual fields in hu-

mans: Its ecological origins and neurophysiological implications. Behav Brain Sci 13:

519–575.

6 of 6 | www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1423673112 Barton and Brewer

Supporting InformationBarton and Brewer 10.1073/pnas.1423673112SI Materials and MethodsAnatomical Data. Scanning was conducted at the University ofCalifornia, Irvine, on the 3T Philips Achieva MR Scanner with aneight-channel sensitivity encoding (SENSE) imaging head coil.One high-resolution whole-brain anatomical dataset was acquiredfor each subject [T1-weighted 3D magnetization-prepared rapidacquisition with gradient echo (MPRAGE), 1-mm3 voxels, rep-etition time (TR) = 8.4 ms, echo time (TE) = 3.7 ms, flip = 8°,SENSE factor = 2.4]. We used custom software (mrVista fromStanford University; white.stanford.edu/software) (1) to segmentwhite matter, which was hand-edited to minimize segmentationerrors. Gray matter was grown from the segmented white matterto form a 2- to 4-mm layer covering the white matter surface.The cortical surface was then represented as a mesh at thewhite-/gray-matter border, which was used to render a smooth3D cortical surface and flatten the cortical representation forvisualization of the visual field maps (2).In addition, one anatomical in-plane image was acquired be-

fore each set of functional scans with the same slice prescriptionas the functional scans but a slightly higher spatial resolution (1 ×1 × 3-mm voxels). These anatomical image slices were physicallyin register with the functional image slices to align the functionaldata with the high-resolution anatomical data. This alignmentwas performed by first, a manual coregistration and then, a sem-iautomated 3D coregistration algorithm, which used a mutualinformation method (3, 4).

Functional Data. The same 3T scanner was used to collect thefunctional MR data, with ∼35 axial slices oriented approximatelyparallel to the calcarine sulcus (T2-weighted, gradient echo im-aging, TR = 2 s, TE = 30 ms, flip = 90°, SENSE factor = 1.7,reconstructed voxel size of 1.875 × 1.875 × 3 mm, no gap). Weanalyzed fMRI data using the same custom Matlab mrVISTAsoftware. For each subject, data in each fMRI session were an-alyzed voxel by voxel with no spatial smoothing. The mean valuemaps of the blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signals wereexamined for potential head movements. No motion correctionalgorithm was applied here, because all scans had less than onevoxel of head motion. The time series from each scan was high-pass filtered to remove low-frequency sources of physiologicalnoise and averaged together to form one mean time series foreach subject, which was then used in the pRF model analysis (5).

Stimulus Presentation. Stimuli were generated using the Psycho-physics Toolbox (6) in the Matlab programming environmenton a Dell Optiplex desktop. Stimuli were back-projected onto ascreen at the head end of the bore of the magnet by a ChristieDLV1400-DX DLP Projector (spatial resolution: 1,024 × 768pixels, refresh rate: 60 Hz). Subjects viewed the display on anangled front surface mirror mounted on the head coil close tothe eyes with a viewing distance of ∼70 cm. Head movementswere minimized with padding and tape. Photopic conditions(maximum luminance = 140 cd/m2) consisted of our standardsetup for visual field mapping experiments, with lights of thescanning room turned on and a neutral density filter over theprojector’s wave guide. Scotopic conditions (maximum lumi-nance = 0.003 cd/m2) during scanning were created by blackingout all light sources in the scanner room and placing additionalneutral density filters over the projector’s wave guide. Subjectswere dark-adapted for 35–40 min before any set of scotopic scansand light-adapted for at least 10 min before any set of photopicscans (7, 8). We also verified dark adaptation at the start of each

scotopic scan session by testing each subject’s inability to per-ceive stimuli within the central 2° radius from fixation (i.e.,within the rod scotoma).

Visual Field Mapping Stimuli. The moving bar stimulus was com-prised of achromatic (mean luminance ∼50 cd/m2) dynamiccheckerboard contrast patterns (∼90% contrast) and spanned avisual field subtending a maximum radius of 11° of visual angle(Fig. 2F). The contrast pattern of the bar aperture consisted ofrows that appeared to be moving in the opposite direction toadjacent rows, with each column spanning the length of the baraperture and each row spanning its width. The bar apertureswere displaced in discrete steps every 2 s in synchrony withthe fMRI volume acquisition. Modulation of the checkerboardcontrast pattern was metameric to modulation of a 500-nm light.The contrast pattern motion created a 2-Hz temporal frequency,and the motion direction changed randomly every 2–3 s. Fourbar orientations (0°, 45°, 90°, and 135° from vertical) with twomotion directions orthogonal to each orientation were used. Thisstimulus set produced eight different bar configurations and atotal presentation time of 192 s at one cycle per scan. Four meanluminance periods were inserted in the last 12 s of each 48-speriod at a frequency of four cycles per scan. To aid fixationunder scotopic conditions, subjects maintained fixation on one oftwo large central crosses, which alternated between spanningeither the diagonals from the corners of the field of view or themidpoints of each of the sides of the field of view. The same twoalternating fixation crosses were used in both scotopic and photopicluminance conditions. The lines of each fixation cross were roughly0.5° wide, and they randomly switched between the two positionsevery 2–4 s as a drifting bar moved passed across the visual field.Subjects attended to these moving bar apertures and respondedwith a button press to an intermittent, subtle change in the motiondirection of the checkerboard pattern (not in sync with the visualstimulus position changes or mean luminance periods).

Assessment of Fixation Stability. Subjects were required to fixatetheir eyes under photopic conditions, where the fixation cross isclearly visible at the center of vision, and scotopic conditions,where the center of the fixation cross overlaps the rod scotoma.As a result, we checked the possibility that differences in fixationstability may have affected our results. Previous work modelingthe effects of eye movements shows that eye movements wouldhave relatively uniform effects across the entire visual field andthat the measurements of visual field maps using pRF modelingremain relatively unaffected by artificial or central scotomas thatcould produce a small to moderate range of eye movements (9–12). Each of our effects is only over a portion of the visual field(either central or peripheral eccentricities). As a precaution, ourfixation stimuli were specifically designed to minimize differ-ences in eye movements and fixation difficulty between condi-tions by being large and extending to the borders of the field ofview (13). Additionally, the fixation cross changed from a large Xshape to a large + shape at jittered intervals to diminish con-sistent effects of fixation stability and additional aid fixation. Oursubjects also underwent extensive training and practice with ourstimuli under both photopic and scotopic conditions as well asother many other studies that require fixation. Subjects who areexperts at fixating, such as our subjects were, perform better thannonexperts at fixation tasks (9, 14).Eye tracking was provided by an MRI-compatible long-range

remote tracking system (Applied Science Laboratories). Any

Barton and Brewer www.pnas.org/cgi/content/short/1423673112 1 of 8

scans with excessive eye movements were discarded from addi-tional analysis (<1% of all scans). To confirm that eye move-ments did not differ between photopic and scotopic conditions,additional analyses were performed. The degree to which eyemovements occurred during a scan can be measured as thevariability of BOLD modulation in the eyes (Eyes ROI) (9). Ifthe SD of the BOLD signal in the Eyes ROI across subjectsbetween photopic and scotopic conditions was significantly dif-ferent, it would indicate that there were differences in eyemovements (Fig. S4 and Table S1). However, a paired samples ttest reveals no difference between photopic and scotopic con-ditions [t (2) = −0.318, P = 0.781], indicating that eye movementsdid not significantly vary between conditions and thus, did notsignificantly contribute to our results. For comparison, scotopiceye movements were compared with an eye saccade task, wheresubjects were asked to make a series of saccadic eye movementsfrom fixation to positions in the midperiphery of the presentlymeasured visual field. As expected, a paired samples t test re-vealed a significant difference between scotopic and eye saccadescans [t (2) = −5.070, P = 0.037], indicating that eye movementswere not a significant factor in scotopic scans.To rule out the possibility that the difference between scotopic

and eye saccade scans in the Eyes ROI was caused by some otherfactor unrelated to eye movements, we compared the SD ofBOLD modulation in V1 between the conditions (Fig. S4 andTable S1). As expected, a paired samples t test reveals no dif-ference between scotopic and eye saccade scans [t (2) = −2.339,P = 0.144], indicating that the difference observed between thescans in the Eyes ROI is related to the difference in eye move-ments between the conditions. Similarly, there was no differencebetween photopic and scotopic scans in the SD of BOLDmodulation in V1, which was revealed by a paired samples t test[t (2) = 0.574, P = 0.624].In sum, although it was unlikely that differences in fixation may

have influenced our results, these measurements confirm thatthere was no significant difference in eye movements betweenluminance conditions.

pRF Modeling Analysis. We used the pRF modeling method toestimate the V1, V2, V3, hV4, and VO-1 visual field maps andpRFs. The pRF for a particular voxel is defined as the region ofvisual space that preferentially activates that cortical site (com-plete details are in ref. 5). In each voxel, the BOLD response toour stimuli was predicted using a 2D Gaussian pRF model withparameters of preferred center location (x, y) and size (spread;σ). The predicted fMRI time series was calculated by convolvingthe model pRF with the stimulus sequence and BOLD hemo-dynamic response function (15, 16). The pRF parameters for

each voxel minimized the sum of squared errors between thepredicted and observed fMRI time series for the bar apertures.Each voxel was independently evaluated in terms of the vari-

ance of the time series explained by the best-fitting model. In thetypical traveling wave measurement of visual field maps, eachvoxel is independently assigned a coherence value, which is equalto the amplitude of the BOLD signal modulation at the stimulusfrequency divided by the square root of the power of the BOLDmodulation at all other frequencies except the first and secondharmonics. pRF modeling uses percentage variance explained asa primary measurement of goodness of fit; here, we convert tocoherence values for comparison with typical phase-encodedtraveling wave visual field mapping studies (5, 17–19). Only voxelswith coherence values exceeding 0.20 corresponding to thatvoxel’s peak response to the stimuli presented were consideredfor additional analysis (20, 21). We have measured the noise invisual cortex using baseline measurements in early visual cortexwith a combination of approaches, including photopic and sco-topic visual stimuli (bars, wedges, and rings) with traveling waveand pRF modeling methods. Our measurements show maximumbaseline noise levels for coherence (from traveling wave mea-surements) of 0.15 and variance explained (from pRF modelingmeasurements) of 0.03.Eccentricity [

ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiðx2 + y2Þ

p] and angle [tan−1ðy=xÞ] were derived

from the 2D Gaussian models and are plotted on the unfoldedcortical surface measured in each subject (Fig. 2 and Fig. S2).The pRF model prediction assumed full stimulation of the visualfield out to 11° radius; the model was not constrained by theexpected presence of the rod scotoma (11). The sizes of pRFs(σ in degrees of visual angle) are presented as a function ofeccentricity collapsed across subjects (Fig. S7).

Definition of Visual Field Maps. We define a visual field map as acomplete map by the following criteria: (i) it represents a com-plete contralateral hemifield of visual space (visual field mapsvary in the degree to which their pRFs extend into ipsilateralspace, and therefore, we ignore the extent of ipsilateral repre-sentation in this definition; also, we group the discontiguousV2 and V3 dorsal/ventral quarterfields into complete hemifieldrepresentations), (ii) both a polar angle and an eccentricity re-presentation must be present, and (iii) the polar angle and ec-centricity representations are orthogonal to one another (22).When presented with reversals in polar angle or eccentricityrepresentations, which denote the borders between visual fieldmaps, we split the reversal evenly between the two maps. Here,we follow widely established conventions for the definitions ofthe posterior and ventral occipital visual field maps V1, V2, V3,hV4, and VO-1 (Fig. 2 and Fig. S2) (19, 21, 23–25).

1. Teo PC, Sapiro G, Wandell BA (1997) Creating connected representations of corticalgray matter for functional MRI visualization. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 16(6):852–863.

2. Wandell BA, Chial S, Backus BT (2000) Visualization and measurement of the corticalsurface. J Cogn Neurosci 12(5):739–752.

3. Maes F, Collignon A, Vandermeulen D, Marchal G, Suetens P (1997) Multimodalityimage registration by maximization of mutual information. IEEE Trans Med Im-aging 16(2):187–198.

4. Nestares O, Heeger DJ (2000) Robust multiresolution alignment of MRI brain volumes.Magn Reson Med 43(5):705–715.

5. Dumoulin SO, Wandell BA (2008) Population receptive field estimates in human visualcortex. Neuroimage 39(2):647–660.

6. Pelli DG (1997) The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transformingnumbers into movies. Spat Vis 10(4):437–442.

7. Barlow HB, Sparrock JM (1964) The role of afterimages in dark adaptation. Science144(3624):1309–1314.

8. Hecht S, Haig C, Wald G (1935) The dark adaptation of retinal fields of different sizeand location. J Gen Physiol 19(2):321–337.

9. Beauchamp MS (2003) Detection of eye movements from fMRI data. Magn ResonMed 49(2):376–380.

10. Kimmig H, et al. (2001) Relationship between saccadic eyemovements and cortical activityas measured by fMRI: Quantitative and qualitative aspects. Exp Brain Res 141(2):184–194.

11. Haak KV, Cornelissen FW, Morland AB (2012) Population receptive field dynamics inhuman visual cortex. PLoS ONE 7(5):e37686.

12. Levin N, Dumoulin SO, Winawer J, Dougherty RF, Wandell BA (2010) Cortical mapsand white matter tracts following long period of visual deprivation and retinal imagerestoration. Neuron 65(1):21–31.

13. Thaler L, Schütz AC, Goodale MA, Gegenfurtner KR (2013) What is the best fixationtarget? The effect of target shape on stability of fixational eye movements. Vision Res76:31–42.

14. Guzman-Martinez E, Leung P, Franconeri S, Grabowecky M, Suzuki S (2009) Rapideye-fixation training without eyetracking. Psychon Bull Rev 16(3):491–496.

15. Boynton GM, Engel SA, Glover GH, Heeger DJ (1996) Linear systems analysis offunctional magnetic resonance imaging in human V1. J Neurosci 16(13):4207–4221.

16. Friston KJ, et al. (1998) Event-related fMRI: Characterizing differential responses.Neuroimage 7(1):30–40.

17. Amano K, Wandell BA, Dumoulin SO (2009) Visual field maps, population receptivefield sizes, and visual field coverage in the human MT+ complex. J Neurophysiol102(5):2704–2718.

18. Brewer AA, Barton B (2014) Visual cortex in aging and Alzheimer’s disease: Changesin visual field maps and population receptive fields. Front Psychol 5:74.

19. Wandell BA, Dumoulin SO, Brewer AA (2007) Visual field maps in human cortex.Neuron 56(2):366–383.

20. Baseler HA, et al. (2002) Reorganization of human cortical maps caused by inheritedphotoreceptor abnormalities. Nat Neurosci 5(4):364–370.

21. Brewer AA, Liu J, Wade AR, Wandell BA (2005) Visual field maps and stimulus se-lectivity in human ventral occipital cortex. Nat Neurosci 8(8):1102–1109.

Barton and Brewer www.pnas.org/cgi/content/short/1423673112 2 of 8

22. Brewer AA, Barton B (2012) Visual field map organization in human visual cortex.Visual Cortex—Current Status and Perspectives, eds Molotchnikoff S, Rouat J (InTech,New York), pp 29–60.

23. Dougherty RF, et al. (2003) Visual field representations and locations of visual areasV1/2/3 in human visual cortex. J Vis 3(10):586–598.

24. Wandell BA, Brewer AA, Dougherty RF (2005) Visual field map clusters in humancortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 360(1456):693–707.

25. Wandell BA, Winawer J (2011) Imaging retinotopic maps in the human brain. VisionRes 51(7):718–737.

Fig. S1. pRF model fits for a single typical V1 voxel at the rod scotoma edge. The measurements for A and B were taken under photopic conditions from thesame voxel at the edge of the rod scotoma in V1 of subject 3, whereas C and D were under scotopic conditions. A and C show representations of visual spaceand the portion of it represented by the population of neurons in this V1 voxel (the result of the pRF model fit). Red/yellow colors depict the regions of visualspace represented by the example voxel. Green color represents no response. B and D show the percentage of BOLD modulation over time. The black dottedlines represent actual data; the solid blue lines represent the pRF model fit. The variance explained (Var. Expl.) by each model fit is displayed above each graph.Note the correspondence between the pRF model fit and the data in each case. dva, Degrees of visual angle.

Fig. S2. Eccentricity maps in photopic and scotopic conditions. (A–F) Pseudocolor overlays on a flattened representation of occipital cortexes from threesubjects represent the eccentricity position in visual space that produces the strongest response at that cortical location. Data for subject S2 are presented inFig. 2. (A, C, and E) Photopic measurements. (B, D, and F) Scotopic measurements. Boundaries of visual field maps are depicted with dotted (boundaries alongpolar angle reversals) and solid (boundaries along eccentricity reversals and the edge of measurement) black lines. Coherence ≥ 0.20. (Scale bar: 1 cm along theflattened cortical surface.) (G, Upper) Color legend represents the visual field from 0° to 11° radius. (G, Lower) Moving bar stimulus for visual field map and pRFmeasurements comprised a set of contrast-reversing checkerboard patterns at eccentricities from 0° to 11° radius. One frame is shown for the bar stimulussequence. Four bar orientations (0°, 45°, 90°, and 135° from vertical) with two motion directions orthogonal to each orientation were used, producing eightdifferent bar configurations. (H, Upper) Anatomical orientation legend. (H, Lower) Inflated 3D representation of a medial view of a representative lefthemisphere from which all data were taken. The black dotted line indicates the region near the calcarine sulcus of the occipital lobe, where the maps weremeasured.

Barton and Brewer www.pnas.org/cgi/content/short/1423673112 3 of 8

Fig. S3. Models to explain ectopic responses in scotoma projection zones (SPZs). (A) Model 1 assumes that ectopic responses of neurons with RFs in the SPZ arenot an aspect of the normal RF organization of the visual system but happen only after extensive cortical reorganization. Model 2 assumes that ectopic re-sponses of neurons with RFs in the SPZ are the expected response of the normal RF organization of the visual system. The key time point to differentiatebetween these two models is immediately after the onset of a scotoma, which for all scotomas induced by damage, is contaminated by stunning of cells in andaround the lesion site. The rod scotoma is a naturally occurring, reversible scotoma that that can be applied noninvasively to a primate without inducingdamage and rendering immediate measurements of RFs interacting with the SPZ uninterpretable. (B–E) Potential confounds in measurements of long-termreorganization and short-term adaptation. Upper represents the RF of a cortical neuron or the pRF of a single voxel, which is completely eclipsed by a retinalscotoma. Lower represents the RF of a cortical neuron or the pRF of a single voxel, which is partially eclipsed by a retinal scotoma. In each panel, solid linesindicate an active cortical (p)RF, and dotted lines indicate a silenced, original cortical (p)RF. B represents the original cortical (p)RFs, with dots as the preferredcenters and circles as the spreads. (C) Here, both (p)RFs are silenced because of a combination of the retinal scotoma and the adjacent retinal stunning, whichoccurs in studies involving the creation of a direct lesion to the retinae (1–3). This combination of scotoma and retinal stunning effectively broadens the si-lenced cortical region. D represents the new effective cortical (p)RFs after recovery from retinal stunning (∼2 wk in retinal lesion studies using photocoagu-lation) but before extensive long-term reorganization. Note that (Upper) the totally eclipsed cortical (p)RF remains silenced, whereas (Lower) the partiallyeclipsed (p)RF is active for the portion of the p(RF) that was silenced by retinal stunning. Such recovery from retinal stunning is impossible to differentiate atthis point from long-term cortical reorganization. In addition, D also depicts the immediate measurements of scotomas, such as the rod scotoma presentedhere or artificial scotomas (4), which do not involve retinal stunning. This type of short-term (p)RF change must be accounted for in studies of long-termreorganization. E represents examples of potential long-term reorganization for these cortical (p)RFs. In Upper, the cortical (p)RF that was initially fully coveredby the scotoma has now shifted to represent new regions of visual space. However, in Lower, this same shift into new territory cannot be distinguished by these(p)RF measurements from the measurements of the expected leftover (p)RF seen in D, Lower.

1. Smirnakis SM, et al. (2005) Lack of long-term cortical reorganization after macaque retinal lesions. Nature 435(7040):300–307.2. Giannikopoulos DV, Eysel UT (2006) Dynamics and specificity of cortical map reorganization after retinal lesions. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103(28):10805–10810.3. Wandell BA, Smirnakis SM (2009) Plasticity and stability of visual field maps in adult primary visual cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci 10(12):873–884.4. Haak KV, Cornelissen FW, Morland AB (2012) Population receptive field dynamics in human visual cortex. PLoS ONE 7(5):e37686.

Barton and Brewer www.pnas.org/cgi/content/short/1423673112 4 of 8

Fig. S4. Eye movement measurements. Each graph represents the BOLD signal modulation in percentage over time for one scan for an individual subject.Measurements for A–C are taken from (D) the Eyes ROI. In general, the more that the subjects moved their eyes, the higher the variations in BOLD as measuredby the SDs for measurements from the Eyes ROI. A paired samples t test reveals that there is not a significant difference in the SD between (A) photopic and(B) scotopic conditions [t (2) = −0.318, P = 0.781], indicating no difference in eye movements (Table S2). For comparison, C represents the BOLD variation in atask where subjects were required to make a series of eye saccades, which has significantly higher SD than in B [t (2) = −5.070, P = 0.037]. Note that a differentscale is used for C than the other graphs; 20% BOLD modulation is indicated for scale comparison. Measurements for E–G are taken from (H) bilateral V1, whichshould not vary between conditions, even if eye movements did. A paired samples t test reveals no difference between (E) photopic and (F) scotopic conditionsin the V1 ROI [t (2) = 0.574, P = 0.624] or (G) scotopic and eye saccade conditions [t (2) = −2.339, P = 0.144] (Table S2), ruling out task differences unrelated toeye movements between the eye saccade and scotopic conditions in (A–D) the Eyes ROI comparisons.

Barton and Brewer www.pnas.org/cgi/content/short/1423673112 5 of 8

Fig. S5. Coherence under photopic and scotopic conditions. (A–E) Each graph displays coherence for photopic (dark gray lines with diamonds) and scotopic(light gray lines with squares) conditions in a single visual field map as a function of preferred eccentricity under photopic conditions averaged across subjects.(A) V1 coherence. (B) V2 coherence. (C) V3 coherence. (D) hV4 coherence. (E) VO-1 coherence. Note the relatively greater drop in coherence for scotopic relativeto photopic conditions in the central eccentricities of each map. Error bars represent SEMs. (F–J) Each graph displays the coherence difference (scotopic − photopic)for each map for each individual subject. (F) V1 coherence difference. (G) V2 coherence difference. (H) V3 coherence difference. (I) hV4 coherence difference.(J) VO-1 coherence difference. The legend indicates the color and marker shape for each subject. Error bars represent SDs.

Fig. S6. Shifts in eccentricity representation across photopic and scotopic conditions. (A–E) Each graph displays eccentricity representation for photopic (darkgray lines with diamonds) and scotopic (light gray lines with squares) conditions in a single visual field map as a function of preferred eccentricity underphotopic conditions averaged across subjects. (A) V1 pRF shifts. (B) V2 pRF shifts. (C) V3 pRF shifts. (D) hV4 pRF shifts. (E) VO-1 pRF shifts. Note that each mapshows significant shifts outward from the rod scotoma in the central eccentricities. Error bars represent SEMs. (F–J) Each graph displays the ectopic eccentricityshift for each map for each individual subject. Positive numbers indicate shifts away from the scotoma (more eccentric from fixation). (F) V1 shift. (G) V2 shift.(H) V3 shift. (I) hV4 shift. (J) VO-1 shift. The legend indicates the color and marker shape for each subject. Error bars represent SDs.

Barton and Brewer www.pnas.org/cgi/content/short/1423673112 6 of 8

Fig. S7. pRF size (σ) measurements across photopic and scotopic conditions. (A–E) Each graph displays pRF size for photopic (dark gray lines with diamonds)and scotopic (light gray lines with squares) conditions in a single visual field map as a function of preferred eccentricity under photopic conditions averagedacross subjects. (A) V1 pRF sizes. (B) V2 pRF sizes. (C) V3 pRF sizes. (D) hV4 pRF sizes. (E) VO-1 pRF sizes. Error bars represent SEMs. (F–J) Each graph displays thepRF size (σ) percentage change for each map for each individual subject. (F) V1 pRF size change. (G) V2 pRF size change. (H) V3 pRF size change. (I) hV4 pRF sizechange. (J) VO-1 pRF size change. The legend indicates the color and marker shape for each subject. Error bars represent SDs.

Barton and Brewer www.pnas.org/cgi/content/short/1423673112 7 of 8

Table S1. Eye movement measurements

Subject

Eyes ROI Control V1 ROI

Photopicfixation Scotopic fixation

Saccadic eyemovements

Photopicfixation Scotopic fixation

Saccadic eyemovements

Mean SEM Mean SEM Mean SEM Mean SEM Mean SEM Mean SEM

S1 1.43 0.11 2.19 0.27 8.40 0.66 0.36 0.02 0.29 0.02 0.59 0.02S3 2.20 0.16 1.38 0.30 5.98 0.25 0.27 0.03 0.30 0.02 1.04 0.06S4 2.35 0.26 2.88 0.32 11.95 1.67 0.35 0.02 0.34 0.05 0.51 0.03Average 1.99 0.12 2.15 0.20 8.78 0.68 0.33 0.02 0.31 0.02 0.71 0.03

Rows for S1–S3 represent measurements of BOLD modulation from individual subjects. Average represents their average. Means are average SDs acrossscans. Data from the Eyes ROI (Fig. S4D) and the V1 ROI (Fig. S4H) are shown. Saccadic eye movement scans were not available for subject 2 for comparisonhere. In general, the more that the subjects moved their eyes, the higher the variations in BOLD as measured by the mean SDs (means) for measurements fromthe Eyes ROI. A paired samples t test reveals that there is not a significant difference in average SDs between photopic and scotopic conditions in the Eyes ROI[t (2) = −0.318, P = 0.781], indicating no difference in eye movements. For comparison, the average SD in the Eyes ROI is significantly higher for an eye saccadetask than scotopic conditions [t (2) = −5.070, P = 0.037]. A paired samples t test reveals no difference between photopic and scotopic conditions in the V1 ROI[t (2) = 0.574, P = 0.624] or scotopic and eye saccade conditions [t (2) = −2.339, P = 0.144], ruling out task differences unrelated to eye movements between theeye saccade and scotopic conditions in the Eyes ROI comparisons.

Table S2. Results of statistical analyses

Map

Central Peripheral

F P F P

Coherence differenceV1 24.347 0.016 4.070 0.137V2 11.327 0.044 6.951 0.078V3 27.564 0.013 5.828 0.095hV4 0.459 0.621 0.412 0.567VO-1 0.698 0.465 0.020 0.897

Eccentricity shiftV1 18.545 0.023 0.133 0.740V2 10.315 0.049 4.295 0.130V3 12.924 0.037 2.009 0.251hV4 11.875 0.041 3.311 0.166VO-1 15.389 0.029 6.040 0.091

pRF size changeV1 8.417 0.062 0.289 0.628V2 0.803 0.436 2.688 0.200V3 4.247 0.131 2.168 0.237hV4 2.147 0.239 0.915 0.409VO-1 24.948 0.015 1.562 0.300

All F values and their associated P values reported are the results ofmultivariate ANOVAs with one hypothesis degree of freedom and threeerror degrees of freedom. Because one test was performed per hypothesis,no correction for multiple comparisons was necessary. Thus, the thresholdfor statistical significance was 0.05 in all cases. See Results for more details.

Barton and Brewer www.pnas.org/cgi/content/short/1423673112 8 of 8


Recommended