+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Bernski - Indirect Effects of Stereotypes

Bernski - Indirect Effects of Stereotypes

Date post: 18-Oct-2015
Category:
Upload: caulfield-holden
View: 10 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
Description:
Indirect Effects of Stereotypes
Popular Tags:

of 33

Transcript
  • The Indirect Effects of Discredited Stereotypes: Social and Political Traits in Judgments of

    Jewish Leaders

    Adam J. Berinsky and

    Tali Mendelberg

    Department of Politics Princeton University

    Princeton, NJ 08544-1012 (609) 258-4750

    [email protected]

    Abstract

    We hypothesize that a stereotype can have an indirect impact over judgment even if some of its pieces are rejected. We test this proposition with a national survey-experiment that describes a hypothetical candidate either as Jewish, Jewish and shady, or neither Jewish nor shady. We find that once cued, a social stereotype trait (shady), even though it is rejected as illegitimate, can activate another, more acceptable political trait (liberal) that historically has been linked with shady. In turn, voters cued with the social trait give more weight to the acceptable political trait in evaluating the candidate, and the candidates support suffers, especially among conservative voters. This indirect influence of discredited stereotypes has implications for our understanding of the way stereotypes influence political judgments and for the ability of groups to overcome a legacy of discrimination. An earlier version of this paper was presented at the Conference on Experimental Methods, Harvard University, May 11, 2001. The authors are listed in alphabetical order. We thank Stephen Ansolabehere, Larry Bartels, James Druckman, James Glaser, and Deborah Schildkraut for their thoughtful feedback, Paul Gerber for dedicated research assistance, Ed Freeland and the staff at the Princeton University Survey Research Center for conducting the study, and Princeton University for a research grant.

  • 1

    The Indirect Effects of Discredited Stereotypes: Social and Political Traits in Judgments of Jewish Leaders

    Introduction

    Increasingly, research on voting has recognized the crucial role that stereotypes may play

    in shaping how voters view and evaluate political candidates. Yet we know little about

    differences in the nature and power of stereotypes. Some stereotypes are well known but

    disreputable and widely rejected. For example, the notions that Jews are greedy, women are

    incapable of governing, and African Americans are born with inferior intelligence were once the

    norm but have since passed from the realm of acceptable social and political discourse. Other

    stereotypes are equally well known but widely accepted. For example, it is widely assumed that

    Jews, women and African Americans are liberal. In this paper we examine the effects of different

    kinds of group stereotypes on voters judgments and evaluations of political candidates. In

    particular, we ask whether stereotypes may have an indirect impact on political judgments

    despite being widely rejected.

    We focus here on the impact of political and social stereotypes about Jews in

    contemporary American elections. The experience of Joseph Lieberman, the Democratic

    candidate for Vice-President in 2000, suggests that stereotypes of Jews probably do not have a

    direct effect on evaluations of candidates, but may have an indirect one. Lieberman was widely

    identified by his own campaign and by the media as an observant Jew. While the coverage of

    Liebermans candidacy was positive, the heavy attention to his Jewish identification, and anti-

    Jewish references in other campaigns, prompt us to ask whether voters stereotypes of Jews may

    present a liability for Jewish political leaders.

    We begin with a general discussion of the nature and functions of stereotypes. We then

    move to a specific discussion of the role played by stereotypes of Jews. At the heart of the paper,

  • 2

    we describe an experiment, embedded in a national telephone survey, designed to test our

    hypotheses. We find that direct cues to social stereotype traits are quite powerful despite the

    illegitimacy of these traits. Voters respond to these cues indirectly, by giving more weight to

    more acceptable, political stereotype traits in their judgments.

    Stereotypes and Political Cognition

    The term stereotype refers to a cognitive structure consisting of a category or label, and

    its corresponding traits.1 These traits are linked together into a coherent structure that resides in

    long-term memory and can become activated ready for use in subsequent judgments (Judd

    and Downing 1995). People use stereotypes in general as heuristics or shortcuts to fill in

    information they lack, to arrive at quick inferences and judgments, and to save the effort of

    mindfulness (Lippman 1922). Stereotype traits may be correct or incorrect, but notably for our

    purpose, even when largely correct, they are over-generalized, resistant to contrary evidence, and

    may be applied in error (Allport 1954; Lippman 1922; Nelson, Acker and Manis 1996).

    Stereotypes of groups, however, may have something more: an affective charge (Hurwitz

    and Peffley 1998, 91; Jussim et al 1995). Often group stereotypes contain cultural baggage

    the traces of a history of group inequality and denigration (Allport 1954; Operario and Fiske

    1998: 34-35). As a consequence, negative traits continue to reside in peoples stereotype of the

    group even after it achieves a significant measure of equality. Though individuals consciously

    reject its validity, the stereotype may be activated nevertheless, and used in subsequent

    judgments, often without intent or awareness (Banaji and Greenwald 1994; Bargh 1996; Devine

    1989; Fiske 1998; Macrae, Milne and Bodenhausen 1994; Mendelberg 2001). People can of

    course control the activation of stereotypes they reject, if they are motivated to do so and their

    cognitive resources are not taxed (Devine and Vasquez 1998; Fiske and Neuberg 1990; Monteith

    1 Other definitions include expectancies and knowledge (Hamilton and Trolier 1986: 133).

  • 3

    1993). But overriding automatically activated stereotypes is an arduous task, especially where

    stereotypes of social groups are concerned (Nelson, Acker and Manis 1996; Operario and Fiske

    1998).

    A key feature of group stereotypes is that they are rich cognitive structures whose various

    traits are linked in interconnected associative network structures (Anderson 1985; Lodge and

    Stroh 1993). Once a particular stereotype trait is activated, or brought to working memory, the

    other pieces of the stereotype may become available through a process of spreading activation

    across the stereotypes associative network. Thus, it is possible that once a disavowed trait has

    been activated, it may activate another, more acceptable trait. Consequently, voters may rely on a

    more legitimate component of the stereotype even if they avoid relying on a less legitimate one.

    The result may be erroneous and inequitable decision-making about political figures.

    Social and Political Stereotype Traits

    We are especially interested in two components of stereotypes about groups: social and

    political traits (see also Huddy 1998). Social traits describe social characteristics of groups in an

    exaggerated or over-generalized manner. Many of these social stereotype traits are now

    considered at the conscious level to be violations of the modern norm that groups should be

    treated equally in a democracy (Mendelberg 2001). Nevertheless, the impact on social and

    political judgments of stereotypes about African Americans and women, which contain traits

    such as violent or feminine respectively, is well documented (Gilens 1999; Gilliam and

    Iyengar 2000; Huddy and Terkildsen 1993; Kahn 1994; Kinder and Mendelberg 1995;

    Mendelberg 2001; Hurwitz and Peffley 1998; Sigelman et al 1995; Sniderman and Piazza 1993).

    Political stereotype traits, by contrast, are often consciously considered an accurate

    representation of the way groups behave in politics. For that reason, they are seen as legitimate

    beliefs. Political stereotype traits may refer to the overall liberal or conservative orientation of a

  • 4

    group, or to more specific characteristics such as feminist (Huddy 1998). Research in political

    science has often focused on the impact of political stereotypes of political groups, such as the

    Democratic and Republican parties (Lau 1989; Lodge, McGraw and Stroh 1989; Rahn 1993).

    But recent scholarship suggests that voters link political and social traits in evaluating members

    of social groups active in politics (e.g., using the trait feminine to evaluate a feminist woman

    activist) (Huddy 1998).

    While social and political traits are distinct, they are linked in peoples minds through the

    cognitive structure of the group stereotype. The social and political traits may not be related

    logically. Nevertheless, the two are likely to be associated and linked with the groups name in

    the cognitive structure of the stereotype. When a social trait is activated by a political campaign,

    a political trait may as well, through its link in the group stereotype.

    Voters are likely to view political traits as more legitimate political considerations than

    social traits. Thus, while most voters reject social stereotype traits as illegitimate, they are likely

    to use political stereotype traits. If an illegitimate social trait is activated it may not receive added

    weight in voters considerations because it will be consciously rejected by the individual. But

    and here is the key voters may give added weight to the more legitimate political trait, even if it

    does not apply, because the overall structure of the group stereotype has been activated.

    Social and Political Stereotypes of Jews

    In this paper we explore the political power of stereotypes by examining reactions to a

    hypothetical Jewish politician. As for many social groups, the stereotype of Jews can be divided

    into social and political components. In the U.S., the social traits of Jews as greedy and cunning

    have very old roots and are well known, but they have been widely repudiated as illegitimate.

    The political stereotype trait of Jews as liberals, however, is widely believed and relatively more

  • 5

    socially acceptable.2 If a social trait is cued during a campaign, it may not harm a Jewish

    candidates support directly, but it may do so indirectly by activating the legitimate trait of Jews

    as too liberal. After all, while their contents may be unrelated, the political and social traits,

    together with the category label Jew, are likely to be linked together in the memory of many

    voters. The stereotype of Jews thus might carry a political penalty for Jewish candidates, given

    that many voters reject liberal leaders.

    The Content of Social Stereotypes of Jews

    The social stereotype of Jews has a centuries-old history in the U.S. (Dinnerstein 1994;

    Higham 1955). According to several surveys conducted since the 1960s, it contains such traits as

    clannish, cunning, and greedy (Martire and Clark 1982; Quinley and Glock 1979;

    Selznick and Steinberg 1969). While levels of anti-Semitism have declined dramatically over the

    past several decades, the exact nature and magnitude of the decline is uncertain (Smith 1993).

    But the key for our study is that these social stereotypes of Jews are sufficiently known,

    even if they are rejected by a large majority of voters. While it is rare that such stereotypes come

    directly into play in electoral politics, there are indications that they still exist in regular political

    discourse. On the national level, Patrick Buchanan has made stereotypical references to Jews on

    the social dimension, which have been widely condemned (Chanes 1995, 15; Foltin 1997, 142;

    Foltin 2000, 151-152; Wertheimer 1995, 52). On a local level we have only suggestive

    anecdotes. For example, a recent influx of observant Jews into the small Iowa town of Postville

    provoked a petition signed by 126 voters (in a town of 1,600 residents) to oppose the first Jew

    appointed to the town council and force him to run in a special election. In a newspaper

    interview, the mayor, who opposed the appointment, explained the opposition in classic social-

    stereotypic terms: The Jewish people are not guilt- free on this. And its not totally about

    2 In our study, when asked whether the statement Jews are liberal politically describes Jews, a minority (43%)

  • 6

    religion. The typical Jewish person is going to bargain. The typical Jewish person will delay

    payment. Thats not the Postville way either. 3

    The Content of Political Stereotypes of Jews

    Though social stereotypes of Jews are discredited, political stereotypes are less so in part

    because they appear accurate. Since the beginning of their large-scale involvement in electoral

    politics during the 1930s, Jewish citizens and leaders have taken consistently liberal positions on

    various issues. Today they continue to be unusually liberal on the separation between church and

    state, social issues relating to sexuality, spending fo r social programs, civil rights and

    government aid to African Americans (Cohen and Liebman 1997; Glaser 1997). Throughout the

    1980s, 40% to 50% of Jews identified as liberal, a rate two to three times higher than that of

    Catholics or Protestants (Sigelman 1991).4

    The belief that Jews are liberal is accurate. But if it functions as a component of voters

    overall stereotype of Jews, it might not be used accurately and judiciously in all cases.

    Lieberman, for example, has taken moderate and even some conservative positions on a number

    of policies. As the New York Times reported after the 2000 Democratic Convention, behind the

    traditional Democratic oratory of his convention speech is a senator who has described himself

    as pro-business, pro-trade and pro-economic growth" (New York Times 8/27/00). As a Senator,

    Lieberman actively promoted the interests of the insurance, high technology and health care

    indicated not too well or not well at all. 3In Iowa, Strangers Become Neighbors, by Richard Meryhew, 2/19/01, Star Tribune, p. A1. Other examples from the past include media references in the late 1960s to Sam Massell, the first Jew to be elected mayor of Atlanta, as the Jewish liberal from the synagogue district (quoted in Dinnerstein and Palsson 1973: 19). In New Jersey in the late 1960s two Jewish candidates for school board were criticized by an incumbent board member: most Jewish people are liberals, especially when it comes to spending for education. . . two more votes and we lose what is left of Christ in our Christmas celebrations and in our schools. Think of it (quoted in Quinley and Glock 1979, 178). These remarks were censured by the school board and the local council, but the two Jewish candidates, who had been favored to win, were defeated by a large margin (Quinley and Glock 1979, 178-179). Some prominent political figures of the 1970s made anti-Semitic references, perhaps most notably Richard Nixon, who referred to kikes and left-wing Jews, and believed a Jewish cabal in the Bureau of Labor statistics worked against him (quoted in Dinnerstein 1994, 232). However, Jaher notes, no major American politicians openly courted an anti-Semitic vote (1994, 243).

  • 7

    industries. He has also made a national name advocating traditional morality in popular culture

    and criticizing President Clintons extra-marital affairs. His overall record is much more

    accurately classified as moderate than liberal. If the stereotype of Jews has potency, however,

    people may judge a candidate like Lieberman as more liberal than he really is.

    More generally, the political trait liberal, if it functions as part of a stereotype of Jews

    that exaggerates and oversimplifies reality, may lead to perceptions of Jewish leaders as liberal,

    even when their actual positions indicate that they are not. A leader whose Jewish membership is

    emphasized may be perceived as much more liberal than he is in fact. Since many voters do not

    identify as liberal, being perceived as a liberal is likely to carry a political liability for leaders.

    In sum, while most voters may reject a message that plays on the stereotypic social traits

    of Jews, that message may nevertheless prompt some voters to give greater weight to their

    stereotypic political trait of Jews as too liberal even if the message makes no allusion to the

    political trait. Negative, discredited social traits may not be used in evaluations of the candidate

    because they are deemed an unacceptable basis for judgment. However, the activation of an

    illegitimate social trait may in turn activate the more acceptable political trait. The candidate may

    lose support from the latter even as he escapes a penalty from the former.

    Method

    In our study, we seek to gauge the effects of stereotypes of Jews on perceptions of

    political leaders. For this purpose, we asked respondents to evaluate a fictitious candidate,

    Howard Wilson. 5 Respondents to a national phone survey were randomly read one of six short

    profiles of Wilson (see the Appendix for the exact wording).6

    4Also, during the past several decades Jews have been significantly more loyal to the Democratic party than Catholics or Protestants (Abramson, Aldrich and Rhode 1998, 110). 5 Respondents, of course, were not told that Wilson was not a real politician. 6Note that the three variables are not fully crossed with each other. The survey was conducted by the Survey Research Center at Princeton University. The survey was fielded from early February to April 2001 using random-digit dialing and a national voting-age sample. The sample characteristics are displayed in the Appendix.

  • 8

    The profiles were identical across the six conditions except for the words that related to

    the stereotypical dimensions of Jews (see Table 1). 7 The first dimension concerned the social

    stereotypes of Jews. In the control condition (no-label) Wilson had no religious or ethnic

    affiliation and was reported to be facing a lawsuit filed against him by a business client who

    accuses Wilson of neglecting and ignoring his business practices. In the label-only condition,

    Wilson was identified as Jewish (among a list of other, neutral characteristics), and was

    reported to be facing the same lawsuit by a business client over neglecting and ignoring his

    business practices. In the social stereotype condition, Wilson was identified as Jewish (in the

    same way as in the label-only condition) but was also reported to be facing a lawsuit filed by

    a business competitor who accuses Wilson of engaging in shady business practices to get

    ahead.

    (Table 1 Here)

    This three-treatment social stereotype dimension was crossed with a two-treatment

    manipulation concerning political stereotypes of Jews. One-half of the respondents were given a

    somewhat liberal issue profile of Wilson. Specifically, they were told Wilson has voted to raise

    spending on assistance to the poor, to increase government involvement in health care and for a

    tax relief plan for middle-class families. The remaining respondents were given a somewhat

    conservative issue profile: Wilson has voted to reduce spending on assistance to the poor, to

    decrease government involvement in health care and for a tax relief plan for middle-class

    families.

    These six conditions effectively manipulate three variables. First, they provide or

    withhold the Jewish label. Specifically, two-thirds of the respondents were told that Wilson is

    Jewish; the other third were given no information concerning Wilsons religious practices.

    7A randomization check is provided in the Appendix.

  • 9

    Second, the conditions either introduced or withheld a cue relating to the social stereotype of

    Jews as a group that practices shady business dealings in order to make more money.

    Specifically, one-half of the respondents who were told that Wilson is Jewish (one-third of all

    respondents) were also told that he is accused of being shady.8 Third, we either cued or

    countered the political stereotype of Jews as liberal. In particular, one half of respondents was

    given information indicating that he is; the other half was given information indicating that he is

    a conservative.

    Before we proceed, it is important to address a potential criticism. We did not construct a

    condition that cued the social stereotype without the Jewish label. We are highly skeptical that

    such a condition would, in fact, measure the impact of the stereotypical dimension (greed and

    dishonest business practices) independent of the Jewish identity of the candidate. A person of

    unidentified ethnicity who is accused of using shady business practices to advance his business at

    the expense of competitors may well be perceived as a Jew. We could of course proceed with

    such a condition and simply ask respondents whether they perceived the politician as a Jew,

    Catholic, Protestant, or something else. But this question probably underestimates respondents

    true perception of the candidates Jewish ethnicity. Instead, we have chosen to forego a no- label,

    social stereotype condition and rely on a pattern of evidence to test the link between the social

    stereotype and political perception (see An Alternative Hypothesis below).

    After hearing one of the six candidate profiles, respondents were asked to evaluate

    Wilson in general and to place him on a number of specific political attitude and trait scales.9 For

    comparison purposes they were also asked to place themselves and several real political figures

    on some of these scales (including Joseph Lieberman). At the end of the questionnaire we also

    8It is important to note here that we hold the presence of a potential scandal constant across conditions and vary the type of scandal. 9All items have been used in major data collection efforts.

  • 10

    measured whether the respondent accepts or rejects various stereotypes of Jews (using

    established questions developed for this purpose and extensively tested on national samples).

    Results

    Our main expectation is that the mere invocation of the Jewish label will activate the

    stereotypic political traits attached to the stereotype of Jews. Moreover, we expect that the

    invocation of the social stereotypic traits will increase the activation of the overall Jewish

    stereotype, political traits included, even if the respondent consciously rejects that stereotype.

    Therefore, we expect that 1) the description of the Jewish candidate that cues the stereotype will

    not cause people to judge the candidate in more socially stereotypic terms, but 2) will cause them

    to judge the candidate in more politically stereotypic terms. Specifically, our hypothesis is that

    Wilson will be perceived as more liberal in the Jewish label profile than in the no- label

    condition and more liberal still in the shady Jewish label condition. If the link between the

    social and political stereotypes is sufficiently strong, then we will see this stereotype activation

    even in the profile that counters the political stereotype with information that the candidate is, in

    fact, politically conservative. Real political ramifications may follow. If people perceive a

    socially stereotyped candidate as more politically stereotypical (that is, as more liberal), the

    candidates support may suffer among the majority of voters, who are moderate or conservative.

    The Absence of an Overt Stereotyping Effect

    As a first step, we ask whether the Jewish label, with or without the social stereotype,

    leads voters to think in more socially stereotypical terms about the candidates.10 We expect it

    will not. We regressed the perception of the candidate as greedy and the perception of him as

    dishonest (coded on the 0-1 interval) on a dummy variable for the Label-only condition and

    another dummy variable for the shady condition (see Appendix for wording). Neither the

  • 11

    Jewish label nor the label with the stereotype cue lead people to judge Wilson as either more

    dishonest or more greedy. 11 In all cases, the coefficients are insignificant in both a statistical and

    a substantive sense. Thus, as expected, the experimental treatments do not affect respondents

    willingness to ascribe socially stereotypic traits to Wilson.

    (Table 2 Here)

    Evidence for Indirect Stereotype Effects on Perceptions of Candidate Ideology

    More crucially, however, we expect these ethnic cues to have important political

    consequences through the more indirect judgment of the candidates political ideology. Our

    second step therefore is to test the hypothesis that cuing the Jewish stereotypes through the

    label only and the shady conditions will activate the political stereotypic trait of liberal.12

    Table 2 presents the means and standard errors in each of the six conditions. Regardless of

    whether Wilson is portrayed as liberal or conservative, cuing the Jewish stereotype increases his

    perceived liberalism. Merely identifying Wilson as Jewish pushes the respondents impression of

    the candidate toward the liberal end of the seven-point scale. Adding the shady trait pushes

    Wilson leftward still. Voters, it seems, are indeed quite ready to rely on the political stereotypic

    trait even when provided clear, direct information about the candidates ideology through his

    issue positions.13

    10All the analyses performed in this paper were conducted using only respondents who did not identify themselves as Jewish. Including the 15 Jews in the analyses does not change the results. 11 The OLS coefficients, with standard errors in parenthesis, are as follows: GreedyCandidate = .40 + .02 (.03) Label-Only + .03 (.03) Shady; n = 599, adjusted R-squared = -.002. DishonestCandidate = .40 + .001 (.03) Label-Only - .01 (.03) Shady; n = 623, adjusted R-squared = -.003. 12The analyses used here make use of a seven-point branching liberal-conservative scale, where 7 is the most liberal response, and 1 is the most conservative response. We get the same results if we use only the first branch of the question, and when we use ordered probit instead of OLS. By way of calibrating the results, the ideology ratings of Joe Lieberman and Dick Cheney are 4.97 and 2.52, respectively. 13 The impact of the shady condition is equivalent to the impact of moving from a moderately conservative to a moderately liberal candidate profile in the control conditions. However, we do not wish to over-emphasize the comparison between the impact of candidate ideology and candidate social trait: by design we avoided extreme variance in the manipulation of candidate ideology, making it rather moderate in all conditions, in order to generalize to most contests. We also note that the fact that candidate ideology has an impact suggests that our ideology manipulation worked well voters saw a statistically significant but moderate difference between two moderately different ideological profiles.

  • 12

    Recall that we also manipulated the political stereotype trait we activated it with a

    liberal issue profile or countered it with a conservative set of issues. The question now is whether

    candidate information that counters the political stereotype trait moderates the impact of the

    social trait. If social stereotype traits have limited power, then when we tell voters that the

    candidate holds conservative issue positions, the impact of the social trait should diminish. To

    find out, we regressed perception of the candidates ideology on a pair of dummy variables

    representing the Label-only and shady conditions, separately for the liberal and conservative

    candidate profiles. The results are displayed in Table 3.

    (Table 3 Here)

    Table 3 shows that where perception of ideological position is concerned, a conservative

    candidate is no less susceptible than a liberal candidate to the effects of social stereotyping. A

    liberal candidate is perceived as somewhat more liberal merely for being labeled a Jew, and as

    slightly more liberal still if he is portrayed as a shady Jew (the effects are in the right direction

    but are not statistically significant from the control candidate). Among the conservative

    candidates, the Jewish and shady Jewish candidates are each perceived as substantially more

    liberal than their counterpart in the control condition (Table 3 shows that each is statistically

    significant relative to the control candidate; the difference between them is not statistically

    significant). A counter-stereotypical, conservative description clearly fails to counter the effect

    of the shady social stereotype trait, and may smooth the way for stereotype activation (though

    the last column in Table 3 shows that the differences between the effects on the liberal and

    conservative Jewish candidates are not statistically significant).14

    14 The effects in Table 3 obtain among both liberal and conservative respondents.

  • 13

    An Alternative Hypothesis: Perhaps They are Shady if They are Liberal, Jewish or Not

    However, perhaps what we would interpret as the effect of activating the stereotype on

    perceptions of ideology is a spurious effect. People may not associate the Jewish label and its

    stereotypical social traits with liberal ideology, as we hypothesize. Rather, our respondents may

    instead associate liberals (of all ethnic backgrounds) with dishonesty or greed. Perhaps if we had

    asked our respondents about a Protestant shady candidate, they would have rated him as liberal

    too. In that case, it is not the stereotypic link between social and political traits that causes the

    increase in perception of liberal ideology; it is the independent association between liberal

    ideology and shadiness, ethnicity aside.

    To assess the validity of this alternative hypothesis, we compared peoples perception of

    the candidates greed and dishonesty in the liberal and the conservative control conditions (see

    Appendix for wording). If people associate liberal positions with greed or dishonesty without

    consulting their stereotype of Jews, then perceptions of the candidates greed and dishonesty

    should be higher in the liberal than the conservative conditions that do not refer to Jews.

    The results suggest otherwise. With perceptions coded on the 0 to 1 interval, the

    unstandardized OLS estimate on a dummy variable coded 1 for the liberal control condition and

    0 for the conservative control condition is -.05 for greedy, with a standard error of .04. The

    results for the dishonest item are even clearer; there, the coefficient on the dummy variable is

    .003, with a standard error of .04. The liberal non-Jewish Wilson is perceived to be no more

    greedy or dishonest than the conservative non-Jewish Wilson. There is no evidence for the

    alternative hypothesis that voters associate a candidates greed and dishonesty with liberal

    ideology, his ethnic membership notwithstanding. Thus it seems that the causal story

    emphasizing the link between social and political traits specific to stereotypes of Jews is correct.

  • 14

    The Impact of Candidates Perceived Liberal Ideology on Candidate Evaluation

    More important than movement in perception of liberal ideology is whether candidates

    incur a political penalty from stereotypes. If the stereotype is in fact affectively charged,

    activating it should lead to a negative judgment, not merely to a biased perception of ideology.

    Does the Jewish candidate lose support through his perceived ideology because he is Jewish?

    And if so, is it merely because of his Jewish identity, or only when he is also attacked as a

    socially stereotypic Jew? Finally, does the candidates actual conservative position moderate the

    impact of the social stereotypic cue?

    To find out, we regressed the feeling thermometer for the candidate on perception of the

    candidates ideology in each of the six conditions.15 The common sense of party politics suggests

    that the penalty paid by the Jewish candidate applies primarily among people who do not identify

    themselves as liberal. After all, for liberal voters, the candidates liberal ideology would not be a

    liability (and might be an asset). Not so for conservative voters, for whom the candidates

    perceived liberal ideology should constitute grounds for rejection. To find out whether

    conservative and liberal voters react in these opposite ways, we estimated the model separately

    for liberal and conservative voters.

    (Table 4 Here)

    (Table 5 Here)

    The OLS coefficients and standard errors are presented in Table 4, and the corresponding

    estimated means in Table 5. Recall that many voters tend to misperceive the socially stereotyped

    candidate in more politically stereotyped terms. The results in Tables 4 and 5 suggest that

    conservative voters go the next step and give more weight to their exaggerated perception. Table

    4 shows that in comparison with the control and label-only conditions, the shady trait leads

  • 15

    conservative voters to give much more weight to their perception of the candidates ideology.

    This holds for the liberal candidate (b = -4.78, se=1.42; difference from label-only is

    significant at p < .05) and especially for the conservative candidate (-6.94, se=1.41; differences

    from label-only and from control are each significant at p < .05). It is the cue to the social

    stereotype trait of shady, and not the mere Jewish label, that sets indirect stereotyping in

    motion. The candidates actual conservative positions do little to inhibit indirect stereotyping

    (Table 4 shows that there is no statistically significant difference between the liberal and

    conservative candidates). Table 5 makes clear what these effects mean: the shady Jewish

    candidates support among conservative voters drops substantially when he is perceived as

    liberal. A shady candidate who manages to avoid the perception that he is liberal essentially

    inhibits the process of indirect stereotyping and suffers few ill effects.

    Liberal voters evaluations grow colder with the social stereotype too, though in a

    different way. Tables 4 and 5 show that when the conservative candidate is not Jewish, his

    perceived liberalism is a reason to support him (b = 4.98, se = 1.91). But when he is a shady

    Jew, his perceived liberalism ceases to be a plus and may become a reason to oppose him (the

    coefficient takes a negative sign though it is not statistically significant, b = -1.77, se = 1.68). His

    evaluations plummet from 61 (for a non-Jew) to 34 (for a shady Jew). Again, the mere Jewish

    label carries no statistically significant ill effects relative to the control condition; only the

    shady trait does so. By contrast with the conservative candidate, the liberal candidate suffers

    no ill effects from the social stereotype (Table 4 shows that the difference between the

    conservative and liberal shady candidates is statistically significant). Thus, if a socially

    stereotyped liberal candidate were to try to shift his actual policy positions in a more

    15 The model includes six dummy variables representing the six conditions, and six variables representing the perceptions of candidate ideology multiplied by each dummy variable. We estimated the model without a constant, for ease of interpretation.

  • 16

    conservative direction (and came to resemble the conservative candidate in our study), he would

    risk losing support among liberal voters. 16

    Let us summarize these results.17 We found evidence for the indirect effect of a social

    stereotype cue. Conservatives not only see the shady Jewish candidate as more liberal than his

    non-Jewish counterpart, but also find the candidates liberal ideology much more objectionable.

    The mere label does not carry ill effects; the candidate suffers a more severe penalty for his

    misperceived issue positions only if he is identified as a socially stereotypic group member.

    Notably, the socially stereotyped candidate only suffers the loss of support indirectly, through a

    stereotyped perceived ideology. The process of stereotyping is indirect: the social trait (shady)

    activates a stereotyped political trait (liberal), which in turn decreases support. Without the

    perceived political trait, the social cue does nothing to erode the candidates support. Also

    notable is the inability of the candidate who takes actual conservative positions to escape the

    consequences of indirect stereotyping. Finally, we found that liberal voters also respond to the

    social stereotype cue, but only in evaluating a conservative candidate. Indirect stereotyping

    works most consistently among conservative voters.

    Conclusion

    In this paper we have attempted to gauge whether negative stereotypes of a social group

    may work in a subtle yet potent fashion. We focused on stereotypes of Jews as a difficult test of

    this hypothesis. Surveys have shown consistently that negative stereotypes of Jews have declined

    dramatically in the United States. Yet the results here suggest that when campaigns cue

    16Validating the intuition that liberals prefer someone they perceive as liberal while conservatives prefer the converse, the effects in the control conditions show that the impact of the candidates perceived liberal ideology is negative among conservatives, and positive among liberals. But this effect does not hold among liberal voters evaluating the liberal candidate. That makes for a problematic comparison with the effects on liberals who are evaluating a liberal Jewish or shady candidate. 17 By way of calibrating the effects, one might contrast them with the impact of Liebermans and Cheneys perceived ideology on their respective feeling thermometer ratings (-3.09, se = .56, and 1.33, se = .70 respectively). We get the same results when we recode the feeling thermometer to three categories (positive, 51-100; neutral, 50; negative, 0-49), and stronger results when we focus on voters who gave feeling thermometer ratings other than 50.

  • 17

    stereotypic social traits even those widely discredited we may witness an indirect process of

    stereotyping by which Jewish candidates ultimately lose significant political support.

    This process, we believe, is fundamentally both cognitive and affective. It is cognitive in

    the sense that stereotypic social and political traits are linked together within a cognitive

    structure that resides in memory. The discredited social traits are activated but controlled, while

    the political, more legitimate political trait is activated but not over-ridden. Because the social is

    linked to the political, it can carry an important indirect consequence for political judgments.

    The process of stereotyping, however, is not only cognitive but also affective. Some of

    the social stereotype traits of social groups with a history of disadvantage are negative. We found

    that only when a negative social trait is cued does the target of the stereotype suffer adverse

    consequences; the group label by itself causes no harm.

    Whether our findings apply to Liebermans candidacy is uncertain. Liebermans

    candidacy does not fit our label-only simulation, since his campaign and press coverage

    highlighted not only the Jewish label but also the socially stereotypic trait of religiosity. Neither

    does his candidacy clearly fit our social trait condition. The trait of religiosity, while social, is

    much more positive than the social trait shady. Lieberman was portrayed as a deeply religious

    man with the associated positive characteristics of moral integrity and attachment to the valued

    institutions of family and place of worship. There was a small exception, however, that hints at

    the kind of campaign we simulated here. For a brief time, Lieberman was cast by the Republican

    National Committee as Slumlord Joe for his role in evicting a tenant from a building he

    administered for his rich uncles estate.18 Talk radio host Rush Limbaugh charged him with

    18 The Hartford Courant, 9/14/2000, Records Refute GOP Hypocrite Claim, by Mike McIntire.

  • 18

    hypocrisy in social stereotypic terms (can you spell chutzpah, Joe?!).19 Still, this episode was

    sufficiently brief and marginal that it probably had little impact.

    What we can say with some confidence is that absent positive information about a Jewish

    candidate, intensive attention to a candidates Jewish ethnicity is likely to pose a liability with an

    electorate composed of more than trivial numbers of conservative voters. Moreover, if a Jewish

    candidate were to run against a candidate in the mold of Patrick Buchanan (who has made

    consistent statements unfavorable to Jews), or in an environment rife with tensions between Jews

    and others (as in the Iowa town of Postville, or at certain moments in New York City), the

    candidate may find himself characterized in negative stereotypical terms. It may not be the

    opposing candidate who does so; cues might present themselves in the course of casual exchange

    among ordinary political discussion partners, on talk radio, or in internet forums. Regardless of

    their source, if such cues are available, anti-Jewish stereotypes may well become activated. The

    consequence will likely be the erosion of the Jewish candidates support.

    Notably, however, the process by which Jewish candidates support can erode is an

    indirect one, and thus potentially difficult to counter. Voters do not rely on social stereotypic

    reasons for opposing a stereotyped candidate. In our study, voters did not reject the candidate

    because he was a dishonest or greedy person. Rather, voters rely on their misperceived reading

    of the candidates ideology. The cue to the negative social trait sets in motion a process by which

    the candidate is perceived as more liberal, and in turn judged more harshly for it.

    But the temptation that this presents for candidates to inoculate themselves by moving

    rightward is probably a false one. Most likely, a candidates attempt to present a moderately

    conservative policy profile will fail, and may even backfire. The results presented here suggest

    that moderately conservative candidates can no more escape the effects of stereotyping than can

    19http://www.rushonline.com/visitors/goreism.htm

  • 19

    moderately liberal candidates. Thus the strategy that may seem most sensible is unlikely to

    succeed.

    A strategy that these results can recommend takes as its point of departure the indirect

    nature of the stereotype process. If voters come to understand that their response derives from

    stereotypical social traits that they reject, they may be more motivated to override the stereotype.

    The literature on stereotypes provides some hope in this regard. When people are motivated to

    interfere with the activation of their own stereotypes, they can often weaken the impact of their

    stereotypes on their judgments, if not completely eradicate it (e.g., Bodenhausen and Macrae

    1998; Devine 1989; for a somewhat more pessimistic conclusion, see Nelson, Acker and Manis

    1996).

    Today, members of social groups with a history of disadvantage are protected from harm

    by virtue of their citizenship in a democracy. But those among them who seek to join the

    governing class may, under specific circumstances, continue to face obstacles rooted in social

    suspicion. The key to the remedy is to understand those circumstances and to investigate the

    ways in which they might be neutralized.

  • 20

    References Abramson, Paul, John Aldrich and David Rhode. 1998. Change and Continuity in the 1996

    Elections. Washington, DC: Congressional Quarterly Press. Allport, Gordon W. 1954. The Nature of Prejudice. Cambridge: Addison-Wesley Publishing

    Company. Anderson, John R. 1985. Cognitive Psychology and Its Implications, 2nd ed. New York: W.H.

    Freeman. Banaji, M. R., and A. G. Greenwald. 1994. Implicit Stereotyping and Prejudice. In The

    Psychology of Prejudice: The Ontario Symposium, ed. M.P. Zanna and J.M. Olson. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Bargh, John A. 1996. Automaticity in Social Psychology. In Social Psychology: Handbook of

    Basic Principles, ed. E. Tory Higgins and Arie W. Kruglanski. New York: Guilford Press.

    Chanes, Jerome A. 1995. Antisemitism and Jewish Security in America Today: Interpreting the

    Data. In Antisemitism in America Today: Outspoken Experts Explode the Myths. Ed. Jerome A. Chanes. New York: Birch Lane Press. Pp. 3-32.

    Cohen, S. M., and C. S. Liebman. 1997. American Jewish Liberalism: Unraveling the Strands.

    Public Opinion Quarterly 61:405-430. Devine, Patricia G. 1989. Stereotypes and Prejudice: Their Automatic and Controlled

    Components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 56:5-8. Devine, Patricia G., and Kristin A. Vasquez. 1998. The Rocky Road to Positive Intergroup

    Relations. In Confronting Racism: The Problem and the Response, ed. Jennifer Lynn Eberhardt, and Susan T Fiske. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.

    Dinnerstein, Leonard. 1994. Antisemitism in America. New York: Oxford. Dinnerstein, Leonard, and Mary Dale Palsson. 1973. Introduction. In Jews in the South, ed.

    Leonard Dinnerstein and Mary Dale Palsson. Baton Rouge: Louisiana State University Press.

    Fiske, Susan T. 1998. Stereotyping, Prejudice, and Discrimination. In The Handbook of Social

    Psychology, 4th ed., ed. Daniel T. Gilbert, Susan T. Fiske, and Gardner Lindzey. New York: McGraw Hill.

    Fiske, Susan T., and Steven L. Neuberg. 1990. A Continuum Model of Impresssion Formation,

    from Attention and Interpretation. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, ed. Mark P. Zanna. New York: Academic Press.

  • 21

    Foltin, Richard T. 1997. National Affairs. In American Jewish Year Book 1997, ed. David Singer and Ruth R. Seldin. New York: The American Jewish Committee.

    Foltin, Richard T. 2000. National Affairs. In American Jewish Year Book 2000, ed. David

    Singer and Lawrence Grossman. New York: The American Jewish Committee. Gilens, Martin. 1999. Why Americans Hate Welfare. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Gilliam Frank D., and Shanto Iyengar. 2000. Prime Suspects: The Influence Of Local Television News On The Viewing Public. American Journal of Political Science. 44:560-573.

    Glaser, James M. 1997. Toward an Explanation of the Racial Liberalism of American Jews.

    Political Research Quarterly 50:437-458. Glazer, Nathan. 1995. The Anomalous Liberalism of American Jews. In The Americanization

    of the Jews, ed. Robert M. Seltzer, and Norman J. Cohen. New York: New York University Press.

    Hamilton, David L., and Tina K. Trolier. 1986. Stereotypes and Stereotyping: An Overview of

    the Cognitive Approach. In Prejudice, Discrimination, and Racism, ed. John F.Dovidio, and Samuel L. Gaertner. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, Inc.

    Higham, John. 1955. Strangers in the Land. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press. Huddy, Leonie.1998. "The Social Nature of Political Identity: Feminist Image and Feminist

    Identity." Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Political Science Association, Boston, September, 1998.

    Huddy, Leonie, and Nayda Terkildsen. 1993. Gender Stereotypes and the Perception of Male

    and Female Candidates. American Journal of Political Science 37:119-147. Hurwitz, Jon and Mark Peffley. 1998. Whites Stereotypes of Blacks: Sources and Political

    Consequences. In Jon Hurwitz and Mark Peffley (eds.), Perception and Prejudice: Race and Politics in the United States. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Jaher, Frederic Cople. 1994. A Scapegoat in the New Wilderness: The Origins and Rise of Anti-

    Semitism in America. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.

    Judd, Charles M. and James W. Downing. 1995. Stereotypic Accuracy in Judgments of the Political Positions of Groups and Individuals. in Political Judgment: Structure and Process, ed. Milton Lodge and Kathleen M. McGraw. Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan Press.

    Jussim, Lee, Thomas E. Nelson, Melvin Manis and Sonia Soffin. 1995. Prejudice, Stereotypes,

    and Labeling Effects: Sources of Bias in Person Perception. JPSP 68: 228-246.

  • 22

    Kahn, Kim F. 1994. Does Gender Make a Difference? An Experimental Examination of Sex Stereotypes and Press Patterns in Statewide Campaigns. American Journal of Political Science 38(1): 162-195.

    Kinder, Donald R. and Tali Mendelberg. 1995. Cracks in American Apartheid. Journal of

    Politic 57: 402-424. Lau, Richard. 1989. Political Schemata, Candidate Evaluations, and Voting Behavior. In

    Political Cognition: The 19th Annual Carnegie Symposium on Cognition. Richard R. Lau and David O. Sears (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Layman, Geoffrey C. 1997. Religion and Political Behavior in the United States: The Impact of

    Beliefs, Affiliations, and Commitment from 1980 to 1994. Public Opinion Quarterly 61:288-316.

    Lippmann, Walter. 1922. Public Opinion. New York: Harcourt, Brace, and Co. Lipset, Seymour Martin, and Earl Raab. 1995. Jews and the New American Scene. Cambridge,

    Massachusetts: Harvard University Press. Lodge, Milton, Kathleen M. Mcgraw, and Patrick Stroh. 1989. An Impression Driven Model Of

    Candidate Evaluation. American Political Science Review 83:399-420. Lodge, Milton and Patrick Stroh. 1993. Inside the Mental Voting Booth: An Impression-Driven

    Process Model of Candidate Evaluation. In Explorations in Political Psychology, ed. Shanto Iyengar, and William J. McGuire. Durham: Duke University Press.

    Macrae, C. Neil, Alan B. Milne, and Galen V Bodenhausen. 1994 Stereotypes as Energy Saving

    Devices: A Peek Inside the Cognitive Toolbox. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 66:37-47.

    Martire, Gregory and Ruth Clark.1982. Anti-Semitism In the United States. New York: Praeger. Mendelberg, Tali. 2001. The Race Card: Campaign Strategy, Implicit Messages, and the Norm

    of Equality. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Monteith, Margo J. 1993. Self-Regulation of Prejudiced Responses: Implications for Progress in

    Prejudice-Reduction Efforts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65:469-485. Nelson, Thomas E., Michele Acker and Melvin Manis. 1996. Irrepressible Stereotypes. Journal

    of Experimental Social Psychology 32: 13-38. Operario, Don and Susan T. Fiske. 1998. Racism Equals Power Plus Prejudice: A Social

    Psychological Equation for Racial Oppression. In Confronting Racism: The Problem and the Response. Jennifer L. Eberhardt and Susan T. Fiske (eds.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Pp. 33-53.

  • 23

    Perdue, Charles W., John F. Dovidio, Michael B. Gurtman, and Richard B Tyler. 1990. Us and Them: Social Categorization and The Process of Intergroup Bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 59:475-486.

    Quinley, Harold E., and Charles Y. Glock. 1979. Anti-Semitism in America. New York: The Free

    Press. Rahn, Wendy M. 1993. The Role of Partisan Stereotypes in Information Processing about

    Political Candidates. American Journal of Political Science 2:472-496. Selznick, Gertrude J. and Stephen Steinberg. 1969. The Tenacity of Prejudice. New York: Harper

    and Row. Sigelman, Lee. 1991. If You Prick Us, Do We Not Bleed? If You Tickle Us, Do We Not

    Laugh? Jews and Pocketbook Voting. Journal of Politics 53: 977-92. Sigelman, Carol K., Lee Sigelman, Barbara J. Walkosz, and Michael Nitz. 1995. Black

    Candidates, White Voters: Understanding Racial Bias in Political Perceptions. American Journal of Political Science 39:243-265.

    Smith, Tom W. 1993. Actual Trends or Measurement Artifacts? A Review of Three Studies of

    Anti-Semitism. POQ 57: 380-93. Sniderman, Paul and Thomas Piazza. 1993. The Scar of Race. Cambridge: Harvard University. Wertheimer, Jack. 1995. Antisemitism in the United States: A Historical Perspective. In

    Antisemitism in America Today: Outspoken Experts Explode the Myths. Ed. Jerome A. Chanes. New York: Birch Lane Press. Pp 33-58.

  • 24

    Appendix I: Candidate Profiles

    Now we are going to ask you some questions to see how voters such as yourself view candidates for public office. I'm go ing to read you a description of a politician. Please listen and remember what you can from the description. Profile 1. Howard Wilson is a state representative who is considering running for an open seat in Congress. Wilson is a 48 year-old businessman who is married with two children. He has served in the state Senate for two terms. While in the Senate, Wilson has voted to raise spending on assistance to the poor, to increase government involvement in health care and for a tax relief plan for middle-class families. As a state senator he worked to expand his state's economic ties with several countries overseas. One potential problem for Wilson is a lawsuit filed against him by a business client who accuses Wilson of neglecting and ignoring his business practices, causing the client to lose a great deal of money. Wilson has denied the charge. Profile 2. Howard Wilson is a state representative who is considering running for an open seat in Congress. Wilson is a 48 year-old Jewish businessman who is married with two children. He has served in the state Senate for two terms. While in the Senate, Wilson has voted to raise spending on assistance to the poor, to increase government involvement in health care and for a tax relief plan for middle-class families. As a state senator he worked to expand his state's economic ties with several countries overseas. One potential problem for Wilson is a lawsuit filed against him by a business client who accuses Wilson of neglecting and ignoring his business practices, causing the client to lose a great deal of money. Wilson has denied the charge. Profile 3. Howard Wilson is a state representative who is considering running for an open seat in Congress. Wilson is a 48 year-old Jewish businessman who is married with two children. He has served in the state Senate for two terms. While in the Senate, Wilson has voted to raise spending on assistance to the poor, to increase government involvement in health care and for a tax relief plan for middle-class families. As a state senator he worked to expand his state's economic ties with several countries overseas. One potential problem for Wilson is a lawsuit filed against him by a business competitor who accuses Wilson of engaging in shady business practices to get ahead, causing the competitor to lose a great deal of money. Wilson has denied the charge. Profile 4. Howard Wilson is a state representative who is considering running for an open seat in Congress. Wilson is a 48 year-old businessman who is married with two children. He has served in the state Senate for two terms. While in the Senate, Wilson has voted to reduce spending on assistance to the poor, to decrease government involvement in health care and for a tax relief plan for middle-class families. As a state senator he worked to expand his state's economic ties with several countries overseas. One potential problem for Wilson is a lawsuit filed against him by a business client who accuses Wilson of neglecting and ignoring his business practices, causing the client to lose a great deal of money. Wilson has denied the charge.

  • 25

    Profile 5. Howard Wilson is a state representative who is considering running for an open seat in Congress. Wilson is a 48 year-old Jewish businessman who is married with two children. He has served in the state Senate for two terms. While in the Senate, Wilson has voted to reduce spending on assistance to the poor, to decrease government involvement in health care and for a tax relief plan for middle-class families. As a state senator he worked to expand his state's economic ties with several countries overseas. One potential problem for Wilson is a lawsuit filed against him by a business client who accuses Wilson of neglecting and ignoring his business practices, causing the client to lose a great deal of money. Wilson has denied the charge. Profile 6. Howard Wilson is a state representative who is considering running for an open seat in Congress. Wilson is a 48 year-old Jewish businessman who is married with two children. He has served in the state Senate for two terms. While in the Senate, Wilson has voted to reduce spending on assistance to the poor, to decrease government involvement in health care and for a tax relief plan for middle-class families. As a state senator he worked to expand his state's economic ties with several countries overseas. One potential problem for Wilson is a lawsuit filed against him by a business competitor who accuses Wilson of engaging in shady business practices to get ahead, causing the competitor to lose a great deal of money. Wilson has denied the charge. [Note: Respondent is randomly assigned to one profile only.]

  • 26

    Appendix II: Sample Characteristics, Variables and Randomization Checks

    Total N = 756. AAPORs RR 3 response rate is 16%.

    20 The 2000 NES, unlike our study, provided an option of havent thought about it much. Most likely, those who selected this answer chose moderate or dont know in our study.

    Comparison of Stereotype Study and 2000 NES

    Variable Stereotype Study 2000 NES

    Party identification Republican 31.5 25.0 Democrat 28.7 34.3 Independent 34.4 27.4 Other/dont know/no response 5.4 13.3

    Ideological self-placement Conservative 53.1 52.3 Liberal 41.6 34.9 Moderate/other/dont know20 5.3 12.8

    Preference on government spending (1 = reduce, 7 = increase) 4.5 4.3 Age (mean) 45.1 47.0 Education

    Some high school or less 7.0 10.0 High school diploma 23.8 28.8 Some college or 2-year degree 28.0 30.3 4-year or more advanced degree 41.2 30.9

    Race White 79.6 86.0 Black/African-American 7.8 13.7 Asian/Pacific Islander 1.5 0.0 American Indian/Native American 1.1 0.0 Other/dont know/no response 10.0 0.3

    Hispanic Yes 6.9 8.3 No 91.8 85.8 Other/dont know/no response 1.1 5.9

    Household income Up to $15,000 8.1 13.3 $15,000 - $24,999 10.2 10.0 $25,000 - $34,999 11.3 11.5 $35,000 - $49,999 17.2 13.7 $50,000 - $74,999 20.6 18.6 $75,000 - $99,999 11.4 7.8 $100,000 or more 9.8 9.9 Dont know 2.0 4.1 Refusal 9.4 8.5

  • 27

    Continued.

    Religious affiliation Protestant 39.4 35.2 Roman Catholic 23.7 23.3 Jewish 2.9 1.8 Other religion 16.5 19.6 No religion 16.1 19.621 Refusal/dont know 1.3 0.4

    Religion, important to respondent Extremely 35.2 38.0 Very 27.0 21.9 Somewhat 23.8 16.2 Not too 12.3 23.6 Dont know 0.9 0.1 Refusal 0.8 0.2

    Female 56.0 56.3 Politically informed

    Blair 45.0 33.9 Rehnquist 36.1 9.6 Majority in the House 62.2 54.6

    Other Variables Used in Analysis

    Note: 7 is the most liberal response, and 1 is the most conservative response

    Note: Candidate evaluation is a 100-point feeling thermometer

    21 The NES asked whether the respondent had an affiliation at all first, then followed up with which affiliation. Our questionnaire, by comparison, provided the no affiliation option in the same question as which affiliation.

    Howard Wilsons Perceived Ideology Conservative Liberal Scale 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

    Percentage of Respondents 9.3 19.1 19.4 3.4 18.8 21.0 9.0

    Feeling Thermometer for Howard Wilson

    Thermometer Ratings

    0-24 25-49 50 51-74 75-100

    Percentage of Respondents 15.2 23.9 40.7 13.6 6.6

  • 28

    Randomization Check The number of subjects is randomly distributed across conditions, with cell size ranging from 122 to 130. Every relevant variable is randomly distributed across conditions with the exception of education. When we included education in our basic models the results were substantially the same as those we report in the text.

    Appendix III: Question Wording and Responses

    1. I am going to read a list of words and phrases people may use to describe Howard Wilson. For each, tell me whether the word or phrase describes him extremely well, quite well, not too well, or not well at all. What about DISHONEST?

    2. We hear a lot of talk these days about liberals and conservatives. When it comes to

    politics, do you usually think of yourself as liberal, moderate or middle of the road or conservative? (IF LIBERAL) Would you say that you are extremely liberal or just somewhat liberal? (IF CONSERVATIVE) Would you say that you are extremely conservative or just somewhat conservative? (IF MODERATE) If you had to choose, would you say that you are a liberal or a conservative?

    3. What about Howard Wilson? Do you think he is liberal, moderate or middle of the road,

    or conservative? (IF LIBERAL) Would you say that he is extremely liberal or just somewhat liberal? (IF CONSERVATIVE) Would you say that he is extremely conservative or just somewhat conservative? (IF MODERATE) If you had to choose, would you say that he is a liberal or a conservative?

    4. Now I'd like to get your feelings toward some of our political leaders and other people

    who are in the news these days. I'll read the name of a person and I'd like you to rate that person using something we call the feeling thermometer. Ratings between 50 degrees and 100 degrees mean that you feel favorable and warm toward the person. Ratings between 0 degrees and 50 degrees mean that you don't feel favorable toward the person and that you don't care too much for that person. You would rate the person at the 50 degree mark if you don't feel particularly warm or cold toward the person. How about Howard Wilson, the person I described earlier? How do you rate your feelings toward him?

    5. Now I am going to read some statements about Howard Wilson, including some that may

    not apply. For each statement, please tell me whether the word or phrase describes him extremely well, quite well, not too well, or not well at all. Wilson is too greedy to be a good politician. How well does this statement describe Wilson?

    6. Now I am going to read a list of statements about Jews, including some that were made

    many years ago. For each, please tell me whether it describes Jews extremely well, quite well, not too well, or not well at all . . . Jews are liberal politically. (Would you say this statement describes Jews extremely well, quite well, not too well, or not well at all?

  • Table 1 Study Design

    Political Stereotype

    Social Stereotype

    Liberal Candidate Conservative Candidate

    Control Condition Profile 1 No Label, Not Shady

    Profile 4 No Label, Not Shady

    Label-Only Condition Profile 2 Jewish Label, Not Shady Profile 5

    Jewish Label, Not Shady

    Shady and Label Condition

    Profile 3 Jewish Label,

    Shady

    Profile 6 Jewish Label,

    Shady

    Table 2 Mean Perception of Candidates Liberalness

    Political Stereotype

    Social Stereotype Liberal Candidate Mean (SE) Conservative Candidate

    Mean (SE)

    Control Condition 4.06 (0.17) 3.30 (0.18)

    Label-Only Condition 4.30 (0.17) 3.87 (0.23)

    Shady and Label Condition 4.49 (0.17) 4.07 (0.19)

  • Table 3 Effect of Candidates Ethnic Label, Candidates Shadiness, and Candidates Ideology on

    Perception of Candidates Liberal Ideology

    Political Stereotype

    Social Stereotype Liberal Candidate

    Coefficient (SE) Conservative Candidate

    Coefficient (SE) Difference (SE)

    Control Condition (constant) 4.06 (0.17)*** 3.30 (0.20) *** 0.76 (0.26)***

    Label-Only Condition (1 = Label condition, 0 = control or Shady conditions)

    0.24 (0.25) 0.58 (0.28)** 0.34 (0.37)

    Shady and Label Condition (1 = Shady condition, 0 = control or Label conditions)

    0.43 (0.24)* 0.77 (0.29)*** 0.34 (0.37)

    N 309 287

    Adj. R2 0.004 0.02

    Note: Perceived candidates ideology is a seven-point scale, 1 = extremely conservative, 7 = extremely liberal. Entries are OLS unstandardized coefficients and standard errors. * = p < .10; ** = p < .05; *** = p < .01

  • Table 4 The Impact of Perceived Ideology on Candidate Evaluation

    Conservative Voters Only

    Political Stereotype

    Social Stereotype Liberal Candidate

    Coefficient (SE) Conservative Candidate

    Coefficient (SE) Difference (SE)

    Control Condition -3.25 (1.30)*** -3.07 (1.36)** 0.18 (1.88)

    Label-Only Condition -0.46 (1.55) -3.31 (1.16)*** 2.85 (1.94)

    Shady and Label Condition -4.78 (1.42)*** -6.94 (1.41)*** 2.16 (2.00)

    N / Adj. R2 308 / 0.87

    Liberal Voters Only

    Political Stereotype

    Social Stereotype Liberal Candidate Coefficient (SE) Conservative Candidate

    Coefficient (SE) Difference (SE)

    Control Condition 0.11 (1.75) 4.98 (1.91)*** 4.87 (2.59)*

    Label-Only Condition 2.82 (1.57)* 1.43 (1.34) 1.39 (2.06)

    Shady and Label Condition 4.03 (1.53)*** -1.77 (1.68) 5.80 (2.27)**

    N / Adj. R2 242 / 0.87

    * = p < .10; ** = p < .05; *** = p < .01

  • Table 5 Estimated Mean Effects of Perceived Ideology on Candidate Evaluation

    Conservative Voters, Estimated Means

    Political Stereotype

    Liberal Candidate Conservative Candidate

    Social Stereotype Perceived Liberal Perceived

    Conservative Perceived

    Liberal Perceived

    Conservative

    Control Condition 40.2 59.7 37.2 55.7

    Label-Only Condition 48.2 51.0 32.1 52.1

    Shady and Label Condition 33.0 61.6 24.8 66.5

    Liberal Voters, Estimated Means

    Political Stereotype

    Liberal Candidate Conservative Candidate

    Social Stereotype Perceived Liberal Perceived

    Conservative Perceived

    Liberal Perceived

    Conservative

    Control Condition 45.8 45.2 61.4 32.2

    Label-Only Condition 62.2 45.3 44.9 36.3

    Shady and Label Condition 59.9 33.9 34.3 45.9


Recommended