+ All Categories
Home > Documents > Intensidad de Terapia y Terapia Inducida Por Restricción_ Control 2

Intensidad de Terapia y Terapia Inducida Por Restricción_ Control 2

Date post: 03-Jun-2018
Category:
Upload: sergio-cruzat
View: 217 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend

of 19

Transcript
  • 8/11/2019 Intensidad de Terapia y Terapia Inducida Por Restriccin_ Control 2

    1/19

    Evidence-Based Systematic Review:Effects of Intensity of Treatment

    and Constraint-Induced LanguageTherapy for Individuals WithStroke-Induced Aphasia

    Purpose: This systematic review summarizes evidence for intensity of treatment andconstraint-induced language therapy (CILT) on measures of language impairment andcommunication activity/participation in individuals with stroke-induced aphasia.Method:A systematic search of the aphasia literature using 15 electronic databases(e.g., PubMed, CINAHL) identified 10 studies meeting inclusion/exclusion criteria. Areview panel evaluated studies for methodological quality. Studies were characterizedby research stage (i.e., discovery, efficacy, effectiveness, costbenefit/public policyresearch), and effect sizes (ESs) were calculated wherever possible.Results:In chronic aphasia, studies provided modest evidence for more intensivetreatment and the positive effects of CILT. In acute aphasia, 1 study evaluatedhigh-intensity treatment positively; no studies examined CILT. Four studies reporteddiscovery research,with quality scores ranging from 3 to 6 of 8 possible markers. Fivetreatment efficacy studieshad quality scores ranging from 5 to 7 of 9 possible markers.

    One study of treatment effectiveness received a score of 4 of 8 possible markers.Conclusion:Although modest evidence exists for more intensive treatment and CILT forindividualswith stroke-inducedaphasia, theresults of this reviewshouldbe consideredpreliminary and, when making treatment decisions, should be used in conjunction

    with clinical expertise and the clients individual values.

    KEY WORDS: aphasia, intensity, constraint, rehabilitation

    Constraint-induced language therapy (CILT), a relatively new apha-sia treatment, (Pulvermuller et al., 2001), has garnered consid-erable interest from consumer groups and clients with aphasia.The major components of CILT involve both forced use of verbal languageand massed practice (i.e., high intensity of treatment). Both are of con-

    siderable importance in light of recent work in neuroscience demonstrat-

    ing that the neuroplasticity of the adult brain can be impacted by several

    experience-dependent principles, including intensity of training and forced

    use of cognitive capacities (Kleim & Jones, 2008). Because intensity of

    treatment is an important variable that affects neuroplasticity, any dis-

    cussion of CILT must consider the impact not only of constraint but also

    of treatment intensity on outcomes. Therefore, the aim of this evidence-

    based systematic review (EBSR) was to examine the current state of the

    evidence that used one or both principles of CILT for individuals with

    stroke-induced aphasia: (a) constraining treatment responses to the verbal

    Leora R. Cherney

    Rehabilitation Institute of Chicago, IL

    Janet P. PattersonCalifornia State University,

    East Bay, Hayward, CA

    Anastasia RaymerOld Dominion University, Norfolk, VA

    Tobi FrymarkTracy Schooling

    American Speech-Language-HearingAssociation, Rockville, MD

    Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol. 51 12821299 October 2008 DAmerican Speech-Language-Hearing Association1092-4388/08/5105-1282

    1282

  • 8/11/2019 Intensidad de Terapia y Terapia Inducida Por Restriccin_ Control 2

    2/19

    modality only and (b) providing treatment on an inten-

    sive delivery schedule.

    The neurobiological principles and rehabilitation

    techniques surrounding CILT were derived from animal

    studies (Taub, 1977) as well as from a series of investi-

    gations of limb movements in patients with chronic stroke

    and hemiplegia (Taub, Miller, Novack, & Cook, 1993; Taub

    & Wolf, 1997). These studies found that motor behavior ofan impaired limb was modifiable with intensive practice

    over a short period of time. The techniques collectively

    known asconstraint-induced movement therapy (CIMT)

    involve restraining the use of the less affected arm for

    themajority of wakinghoursto counteract thepresumed

    learned nonuse of the affected arm. Repetitive training

    and shaping of movementsof the affected arm take place

    in intensive treatment (6 hr per day) over 2 consecutive

    weeks.

    In the past 20 years, a large body of evidence, ap-

    plying either the original CIMT technique or a variant of

    this approach, has accumulated in support of the efficacyof constraint-induced (CI) therapy for rehabilitating

    hemiparetic arm use in individuals with chronic stroke

    (Hakkennes & Keating, 2005; Taub, Uswatte, & Piclikiti,

    1999). To date, there are more than 120 published stud-

    ies, including a single-site, placebo-controlled trial (Taub

    et al., 2006) and a large multisite randomized trial (Wolf

    et al., 2006). Although the magnitude of the treatment

    effects have varied throughout the CIMT literature, the

    generally positive outcomes have encouraged investigators

    to examine the effects of CI and their potential benefits in

    otherdomains of strokerehabilitation, including aphasia.

    Since being introduced to the field of aphasiology byPulvermuller and colleagues (2001), CILT has been met

    with great interest. Pulvermuller et al. argued that pa-

    tients with aphasia often use the communication chan-

    nel that is accessible to them with the least amount of

    effort, such as gestures and drawing, rather than spoken

    language. A constrained approach for aphasia is incor-

    porated in the therapeutic setting by forcing the patient

    to communicate only through verbal channels while lim-

    iting the use of all other communication channels. The

    implementation of CILT in patients with aphasia con-

    sisted of intensive practice through a series of language

    games requiring verbal production of phrases and sen-tences of increasing length that were shaped into succes-

    sively better approximations. Resultscomparing language

    improvements after 2 weeks of CILT to approximately

    4 weeks of traditional therapy indicated significant im-

    provements in the CI group on several standard clinical

    tests, self-ratings, and blinded-observer ratings of the

    patientscommunicative effectiveness in everyday life.

    Two principles of CILT need to be considered when

    interpreting the findings. The first principle, forced use

    of verbal language, requires that all responses provided

    during language treatment activities be constrained or

    restricted to the spoken modality. Nonverbal communi-

    cation attempts such as gesturing, drawing, or writing

    are not allowed and are not considered as communica-

    tion intents because participants are on opposite sides of

    a barrier. The second principle, massed practice, involves

    a high-intensity treatment schedule consisting of 34 hr

    of treatment per day for 2 weeks. Participants in the

    Pulvermuller et al. (2001) study were randomized to aCILT group and a comparison group that received con-

    ventional speech treatment provided on a distributed

    schedule over 35 weeks. That is, the groups differed in

    two dimensions of treatment: the type of treatment ad-

    ministered and the intensity of the treatment provided.

    Thus, it is difficult to determine whether the positive

    treatment results reported for the CILT group as com-

    pared to the conventional treatment group emanate from

    the constrained forced language use, the intensity of the

    treatmentschedule, or a combination of these two factors.

    Intensity of treatment hasbeen a topic of interest for

    some time in aphasia treatment studies (Poeck, Huber,& Willmes, 1989). Findings from Robeys (1998) meta-

    analysis of the aphasia literature reported large effect

    sizes (ESs) associated with treatment provided for 2 or

    more hr per week. In a review focused on intensity and

    outcomes, Bhogal, Teasell, and Speechley (2003) reported

    better treatment outcomes in studies that provided in-

    tensive treatment schedules. On average, the more in-

    tensive treatment schedules equaled 8.8 hr per week for

    11 weeks, compared with the less intensive schedules of

    2 hr per week for 23 weeks of treatment. Although both

    reviews examined the intensity literature, the schedule

    of treatment provided in the cited studies is far less thanthe intensity of services given in CILT.

    This review examines the current state of the evi-

    dence employing the dual principles of CILT for individ-

    uals with stroke-induced aphasia.In additionto analyzing

    and synthesizing the scientific research in this area, the

    authors piloted the use of a newly created levels-of-

    evidence scheme developed by the American Speech-

    Language-Hearing Association (ASHA) National Center

    for Evidence-Based Practice in Communication Disor-

    ders (N-CEP), along with its advisory committee. This

    scheme classifies treatment studies according to several

    quality indicators and the phase of research (Robey,2004). We piloted this system to determine its applica-

    bility to treatment studies in the area of communication

    sciences and disorders and provided feedback to N-CEP

    and its advisory committee prior to the widespread

    adoption of this system.

    Clinical QuestionsTen clinical questions were identified by the authors

    for review (see Table 1). In constructing these questions,

    Cherney et al.:EBSR: Effects of Intensity of Treatment and CILT 1283

  • 8/11/2019 Intensidad de Terapia y Terapia Inducida Por Restriccin_ Control 2

    3/19

    the nature of outcome measurements and stage of re-covery from aphasia were considered. In keeping with

    current ASHA policy documents, such as the Scope of

    Practice in Speech-Language Pathology (ASHA, 2001),

    the World Health Organizations (2001) International

    Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health:ICF

    was selected to categorize the various outcome measures

    representing language impairment (e.g., standard apha-

    sia tests of phonology, syntax, and semantics) or commu-

    nication activity/participation (e.g., functional real-life

    use of language in connected speech measures or com-

    munication ratings scales). Because the stage of recovery

    from stroke-induced aphasiaacute or chronicinfluences

    recovery patterns (Robey, 1998) and studies typically

    include participants in only one of these stages, the clin-

    ical questions also included participants with chronic

    and acute aphasia (distinguished by mean time post

    onset [TPO] 3 months). Therefore, eight clinical ques-

    tions reflect combinations of the following parameters:

    (a) CILT principlesresponse constraint and treatment

    intensity; (b) aphasia chronicityacute and chronic; and

    (c) outcome measures identified from theICFlanguage

    impairment and communication activity/participation.

    Two additional questions were constructed to address

    maintenance of treatment behavior in chronic aphasia

    for a total of 10 clinical questions (see Table 1).

    MethodA systematic search of the aphasia literature was

    conducted to identify studies that directly investigated

    CILT as the specific intervention for aphasia or directly

    compared conditions of higher and lower intensity treat-

    ment for aphasia. Fifteen electronic databases were

    searched: PubMed, CINAHL, PsycINFO, PsycArticles,

    CSA Linguistics and Language Behaviour Abstracts,

    Combined Health Information Database, Health Source:Nursing, Science Citation Index, ScienceDirect, NeLH,

    REHABDATA, Social Science Citation Index, SUMSearch,

    TRIP Database, and Cochrane Database of Systematic

    Reviews. Additional searches were also performed on all

    ASHA journals, the University of Pittsburgh Clinical

    Aphasiology Conference Proceedings, National Institutes

    of Health (NIH) abstracts, and Google Scholar. In addi-

    tion, references from all relevant articles were examined

    to identify other applicable studies. A total of 25 ex-

    panded search terms included those related to stroke-

    induced aphasia, amount and intensity of treatment, and

    CILT. Studies were initially considered for the review if

    they were published in a peer-reviewed journal from 1990

    to 20061, were written in English, and contained original

    data addressing 1 or more of the 10 clinical questions. For

    studiesaddressing a clinical question related to intensity,

    the keyword intensity or amount of treatment had to be

    included in the abstract or the title of the article. In ad-

    dition, studies were limited to those that included adults

    18 years of age or older with stroke-induced aphasia.

    Studies that included individuals with underlying cog-

    nitive deficits or any other primary medical diagnoses

    were excluded. Studies that used pharmacological inter-

    vention for aphasia as the comparison treatment were

    also excluded.

    The initial search schematicized in Figure 1 yielded

    441 citations. Two authors (TF and TS), blind to one an-

    others results, reviewed each abstract and identified

    36 abstracts as meeting the initial inclusioncriteriawith

    91% agreement. Of those preliminarily accepted, 26 were

    Table 1.Clinical questions.

    Question No. Clinical QuestionIntensity

    1. For stroke-induced chronic aphasia, what is the influence of treatment intensity on measures of language impairment?2. For stroke-induced chronic aphasia, what is the influence of treatment intensity on measures of communication activity/participation?3. For stroke-induced acute aphasia, what is the influence of treatment intensity on measures of language impairment?4. For stroke-induced acute aphasia, what is the influence of treatment intensity on measures of communication activity/participation?

    5. For stroke-induced chronic aphasia, what treatment outcomes are maintained following intensive language treatment?

    Clinical QuestionCILT

    6. For stroke-induced chronic aphasia, what is the influence of constraint-induced language therapy on measures of language impairment?7. For stroke-induced chronic aphasia, what is the influence of constraint-induced language therapy on measures of communication

    activity/participation?8. For stroke-induced acute aphasia, what is the influence of constraint-induced language therapy on measures of language impairment?9. For stroke-induced acute aphasia, what is the influence of constraint-induced language therapy on measures of communication

    activity/participation?10. For stroke-induced chronic aphasia, what treatment outcomes are maintained following constraint-induced language therapy?

    1The original purpose of the systematic review was to examine the effects

    of CILT. It became apparent that it would be impossible to adequately

    complete this review independent of addressing treatment intensity, which

    is a key principle of CILT. The search date was determined based on the

    emergence of CI literature in the early 1990 s.

    1284 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol. 51 12821299 October 2008

  • 8/11/2019 Intensidad de Terapia y Terapia Inducida Por Restriccin_ Control 2

    4/19

    rejected upon review of the full text by the author panel,

    as they did not report original data or include direct evi-

    dence of a contrast between conditions (e.g., a more in-

    tensiveconditionvs. a less intensivecondition) pertaining

    to one or more of the clinical questions. Ten studies met

    the final inclusion.

    Each of these studies was independently reviewed

    and evaluated for methodological quality by two of threeauthors (LC, JP, AR). One study written by an author

    (AR) was reviewed by two other panel authors (LC, JP).

    The authors, blind to one anothers results, appraised

    each study on the basis of quality indicators described in

    Table 2. For each indicator, quality markers are ordered

    from highest to lowest level of quality. A study received

    1 point for each quality indicator if the highest level of

    quality was incorporated. All nine indicators were rel-

    evant to studies incorporating controlled trials, leading

    to a maximum quality score of 9. For other study designs,

    where intent-to-treat analysis was not relevant, the

    maximum quality score was 8. Credit for study design

    varied by stage of research (see Table 3). For example,

    efficacy studies using controlled trial designs received

    1 point. Discovery studies received 1 point for research

    design if reporting retrospective case controls or single-

    participant studies. Reliability of scores was assessed,

    and any discrepancies in scoring were sent back to the

    full author panel for resolution. All disagreements were

    resolved via consensus.

    In addition to assessing methodological quality, the

    phase of clinical research was determined using the

    decision tree depicted in Figure 2. The four stages of

    researchdiscovery, efficacy, effectiveness, and cost

    benefit/public policy researchare defined in Table 3.

    A final synthesis of the body of scientific literature was

    compiled into an evidence table on the basis of the study

    quality marker score and corresponding stage of research.

    ESs were calculated for outcome measures whenever

    possible. For group studies, Cohens d (Cohen, 1988) was

    calculated (corrected for bias following Hedges & Olkin,1985). ESs were calculated from group means and stan-

    dard deviations or were estimated from results of anal-

    yses of variance (ANOVAs) or t tests. We used Cohens

    benchmarks for small, medium, and large ESs as .2, .5,

    and .8, respectively. Whether these benchmarks are ap-

    plicable to aphasia treatment studies has yet to be de-

    termined. In the one single-participant experiment

    (Raymer, Kohen, & Saffell, 2006), the authors calculated

    the weighted ESs using the methods described by Beeson

    and Robey (2006). Although Busk and Serlin (1992)

    reported that large ESs in single-participant studies sur-

    pass 2.5, Beeson and Robey noted benchmarks for small,

    medium, and large ESs in single-participant designs as

    2.6, 3.9, and 5.8.

    ResultsOf the 10 studies that met inclusion criteria for the

    systematic review, 5 studies investigated treatment in-

    tensity (Basso & Caporali, 2001; Denes, Perazzolo, Piani,

    & Piccione, 1996; Hinckley & Carr, 2005; Hinckley &

    Craig, 1998; Raymer et al., 2006), 4 studies evaluated

    Figure 1. Process for identification of included studies.

    Cherney et al.:EBSR: Effects of Intensity of Treatment and CILT 1285

  • 8/11/2019 Intensidad de Terapia y Terapia Inducida Por Restriccin_ Control 2

    5/19

    CILT (Maher et al, 2006; Meinzer et al, 2004; Meinzer,

    Djundja,Barthel,Elbert,& Rockstroth, 2005; Pulvermuller,

    Hauk, Zohsel, Neininger, & Mohr, 2005), and 1 study ex-

    amined both (Pulvermuller et al., 2001). These studies

    allowed us to address 7 of the 10 clinical questions (see

    Table 1): Questions 1, 2, 5, 6, 7, and 10, which are related

    to language impairment and communication activity/

    participation in individuals with chronic aphasia, and

    Question 3, which is related to the effects of treatment

    intensity on measures of language impairment for in-

    dividuals with acute aphasia. No study investigated

    CILT or intensive treatment effects for measures of

    Table 2.Quality indicators in the ASHA levels-of-evidence scheme.

    Indicator Description Quality marker

    Study design The type of design used in the study. Controlled trial(Efficacy research)*Retrospective case control (Effectiveness or

    costbenefit research)*Single participant study (Discovery research)*

    Case seriesCase study

    Blinding The practice of keeping investigators or participants ignorant tothe group to which participants are assigned. For the purposesof the critical appraisal,blindingrefers to assessors only.

    Assessors blinded*

    Assessors not blinded or not stated

    Sampling The method(s) used to choose and assign participants to theexperimental conditions in the study.

    Random sample adequately described*Random sample inadequately describedConvenience sample adequately describedConvenience sample inadequately described or

    hand-picked sample or not stated

    Group/participant

    comparability

    How similar the participants/groups were at the start of the

    study or how adequately they were described.

    Groups/participants comparable at baseline

    on important factors(between-subject design)or participant(s) adequately described(within-subject design)*

    Groups/participants not comparable at baselineor comparability not reported or participant(s)not adequately described

    Treatment fidelity The procedure used to ensure that the treatmentwas delivered as intended.

    Evidence of treatment fidelity*No evidence of treatment fidelity

    Outcomes The measure(s) used in the study to quantify improvement. At least one primary outcome measure isvalid and reliable*

    Validity unknown, but appears reasonable;measure is reliable

    Invalid and/or unreliable

    Significance The likelihood that the study findings occurred by chance. pvalue reported or calculable*pvalue neither reported nor calculable

    Precision The size or magnitude of any difference found between thetreatment under investigation and the control condition.

    Effect size and confidence intervalreported or calculable*

    Effect size or confidence interval, but notboth, reported or calculable

    Neither effect size nor confidenceinterval reported or calculable

    Intent-to-treat(controlled trials only)

    Participants are analyzed according to the group to which they wereinitially assigned, regardless of whether or not they dropped out,

    fully complied with the treatment, or crossed over and receivedthe other treatment.If number of participants at pre-treatment =number at post-treatment, study received credit.

    Analyzed by intent-to-treat*Not analyzed by intent-to-treat*

    *Highest level of quality needed to receive 1 point.

    1286 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol. 51 12821299 October 2008

  • 8/11/2019 Intensidad de Terapia y Terapia Inducida Por Restriccin_ Control 2

    6/19

    communication activity/participation in acute aphasia

    (covered in Questions 4, 8, and 9).

    Participants

    Table 4 provides a detailed description of the 141 par-

    ticipants in the 10 studies (e.g., age, education level,

    gender, handedness, TPO, aphasia type, and severity).

    The total sample size (N) of participants reported in the

    studies does not account for the possible overlap of par-

    ticipants across four studies: Pulvermuller et al. (2001

    and 2005) and Meinzer et al. (2004 and 2005).

    For the intensity studies, type of aphasia was pro-

    vided in five studies; 81% (55 of 68) of the participants

    were nonfluent. Severity of aphasia was provided in four

    studies; 40% (23 of 57) of participants tended to have

    more severe aphasia, with 30% (19 of 63) of participantsbeing described as having global aphasia. The mean TPO

    was considered acute in one study (3.1 months in Denes

    et al., 1996);the other studies included participantswith

    chronic aphasia, ranging from 11.2 to 67.6 monthspostonset.

    Similarly for the CILT studies, 60% of participants

    (42 of 70) had nonfluent forms of aphasia, typically de-scribed as Brocas aphasia. Most had moderate aphasia

    (48%, 30 of 62), whereas 19 participants (31%) had mild

    aphasia and 12 participants (19%) had severe aphasia.

    Mean TPO ranged from 36.8 months to 90 months, all in

    the chronic range.

    Four of the 10 studies were conducted in Germany,

    4 in the United States (U.S.), and 2 in Italy. Two German

    studies (Pulvermuller et al., 2001, 2005) included mono-

    lingual native speakers of German. The other two Ger-

    man studies (Meinzer et al., 2004, 2005) did not mention

    linguality of the participants. Two U.S. studies (Hinckley

    & Carr, 2005; Hinckley & Craig, 1998) noted that all par-ticipants were native speakers of English, whereas two

    Figure 2. Stage of research.

    Table 3.Continuum of research stages.

    Stage Description

    Discovery Treatment approaches are developed and assessed in the context of whetherthey show promise of being efficacious.

    Efficacy Promising interventions are tested in a rigorous way under ideal, highlycontrolled conditions to determine the outcome that results.

    Effectiveness The intervention is tested in a real-worldclinical setting. This phaseis often conducted if the intervention demonstrates positive outcomes in ahighly controlled setting of a clinical trial.

    Costbenefit/public policy A study is conducted of the political and economic environment in which theintervention is best delivered. This phase is often conducted once it hasbeen shown that an intervention is both efficacious and effective.

    Cherney et al.:EBSR: Effects of Intensity of Treatment and CILT 1287

  • 8/11/2019 Intensidad de Terapia y Terapia Inducida Por Restriccin_ Control 2

    7/19

    Table 4.Patient characteristics.

    Study N Age Education Gender Handedness TPO Etiology

    Basso & Caporali,2001 (Intensity)

    6 3548 yrs 817 yrs 4 M R 222 months 2 hemorrhagic M= 41 yrs M= 12.5 yrs 2 F M= 11.2 months 4 ischemic

    2 n

    Denes et al.,

    1996 (Intensity)

    17 60.2 yrs 7.2 yrs 8 M R 2.44.5 months 4 hemorrhagic

    9 F M= 3.1 months 13 ischemicHinckley & Craig,

    1998, Study 3(Intensity)

    10 2578 yrs 12+ yrs 6 M 9 R 7 47 months Thromboembolic M= 48 yrs 4 F 1 L M= 20 months

    Hinckley & Carr,2005 (Intensity)

    13 1972 yrs NR NR R 699 months L CVA M= 50 yrs M= 27 months intensive 1

    M= 40 months nonintensive

    Maher et al., 2006 (CILT) 9 4173 yrs 1216+ yrs 6 M 8 R 1472 months L CVA M= 58 yrs 3 F 1 R+ M= 36.8 months

    Meinzer et al., 2004 (CILT) 28 3580 yrs NR 14 M R 12156 months 20 ischemic M

    = 54.6 yrs 14 F M

    = 43.8 months 8 hemorrhagic

    Meinzer et al., 2005 (CILT) 27 1880 yrs NR 16 M NR 13116 months 11 hemorrhagic M= 51.5 yrs 11 F M= 43.1 months 16 ischemic

    Pulvermuller et al., 2001(CILT and Intensity)

    17 4272 yrs 913 years 12 M 14 R 2223 months L CVA M= 54.9 yrs M= 11 yrs 5 F 3 R+ M= 67.6 months

    Pulvermuller et al.,2005 (CILT)

    9 3972 yrs 1013 yrs 6 M 7 R 16223 months L CVA M= 54.4 yrs M= 11.3 yrs 3 F 2 R+ M= 90 months

    Raymer et al.,2006 (Intensity)

    5 5182 yrs 814 yrs 2 M 5 R 442 months L CVA M= 70.8 yrs M= 11.4 yrs 5 F M= 18.4 months

    1

    Note. TPO = time postonset; yrs = years; NR = not reported; R+ = ambidextrous; L CVA = left cerebrovascular accident; NS = not severe; BDAE =Kaplan, 1983).

    1288

    JournalofSpeech,

    Language,andHearingResearch

    Vol.51

    12821299

    October2008

  • 8/11/2019 Intensidad de Terapia y Terapia Inducida Por Restriccin_ Control 2

    8/19

    other U.S. studies (Maher et al., 2006; Raymer et al.,

    2006) did notaddress linguality. OneItalian study (Denes

    et al., 1996) included only native speakers of Italian. The

    second Italian study (Basso & Caporali, 2001) included

    5 participants who were native speakers of Italian and 1

    who was a native of Sweden and completely bilingual.

    The educational level of the participants was reported

    in only 6 of the 10 studies. The mean years of educa-tion varied from 10.9 years (Pulvermuller et al., 2001) to

    15 years (Maher et al., 2006). One study (Hinckley &

    Craig, 1998) reported that all participants completed

    high school or above, and the Basso and Caporali (2001)

    study reported educational attainment on only 4 of 6 par-

    ticipants. Only one study (Hinckley & Carr, 2005) reported

    mean data on socioeconomic status. Those participants

    had a mean score of 2.3 on the Hollingshead Four Factor

    Index of Social Status (Hollingshead, 1975).

    Study Characteristics and QualityTable 5 summarizes information regarding the in-

    tervention, including schedule, amount and duration of

    treatment, andoutcome measures. Most studies provided

    2430 hr of treatment; several studies reported more

    than 100 hr of treatment (e.g., Hinckley & Craig, 1998).

    Treatment schedules varied across studies, as did the

    nature of treatment provided.

    Table 6 displays quality marker ratings obtained for

    the studies. All 10 studies described participants suf-

    ficiently and reported data in a manner in which statis-

    tical significance was available. Studies were lacking in

    several areas, however, including useof a clearly describedrandomized assignment scheme for participants in group

    designs, provided in only one study; blinding of assessors

    to treatment conditions, indicated in only two studies; and

    providing evidence of treatment fidelity, also noted in

    only two studies.

    Four studies were judged as discovery phase re-

    search and five studies as efficacy research. Only one

    study fell into the effectiveness stage of research, and

    none addressed costbenefit or public policy.

    Intensity ResultsTable 7 displays stage of research, quality scores,

    and ESs for the six studies examining treatment inten-

    sity. These studies addressed Questions 1, 2, 3, and 5

    (see Table 1). No data were available to answer Ques-

    tion 4. Five of these studies contained sufficient data

    for calculation of treatment ESs. The effect of intensity

    in Denes et al. (1996), Pulvermuller et al. (2001), and

    Hinckley andCarr (2005) wasderivedfrombetween-group

    comparisons for groups receiving intensive and non-

    intensive treatment. The effect of intensity in Study 3 of

    Hinckley andCraig (1998) wasderived from within-group

    comparisons of the pre- and post difference scores from

    each intensive 6-week training session compared with the

    nonintensive 6-week training session. In Raymer et al.

    (2006), the effects came from within-subject comparisons

    across the individual participants.

    Clinical Question 1: For stroke-induced chronic apha-

    sia, what is the influence of treatment intensity on mea-

    sures of language impairment? Four group studies usedimpairment outcome measures for which eight ESs were

    calculable, including seven large ESs, all in favor of more

    intensive treatment. In the single-participant design of

    Raymer et al. (2006), ESs were larger in the more inten-

    sive condition for picture-naming acquisition and larger

    in the less intensive condition for word/picture verifica-

    tion. ESs could not be calculated for Basso and Caporali

    (2001), who described case studies of three pairs of indi-

    viduals. Individuals receiving more intensive treatment

    showedgreatergains on language impairment tasks than

    didthecomparison individualswhoreceiveda lessintensive

    schedule. Thus, the language impairment outcome mea-sures favored more intensive treatment for all language

    measures except one in participants with chronic aphasia.

    Clinical Question 2: For stroke-induced chronic apha-

    sia, what is the influence of treatment intensity on mea-

    sures of communication activity/participation? Three

    group studies used communication measures for which

    nine ESs were calculable, including content unit (CU)

    analysis, oral and written responses on a catalogue order-

    ing task administered in quiet and concurrent noise con-

    ditions, and the Communication Activities of Daily Living

    (CADL2; Holland,Frattali, & Fromm, 1999). ESsranged

    from 1.15 to 3.78, with five favoring more intensive treat-ment (four large effects) and four favoring less intensive

    treatment (two large effects).

    Basso and Caporali (2001) also contributed data to

    this clinical question, as they described greater changes

    in participants conversational abilities in all persons re-

    ceiving more intensive treatment as compared with the

    counterpart receiving less intensive treatment. Overall,

    findings for outcome measures of communication activity/

    participation were mixed, with some favoring less in-

    tensive treatment and others favoring more intensive

    treatment.

    Clinical Question 3: For stroke-induced acute aphasia,

    what is the influence of treatment intensity on measures of

    language impairment? In one study of 17 individuals,

    (Denes et al., 1996), the ESs for the Aachen Aphasia Test

    (AAT; Huber, Poeck, & Williams, 1984) ranged from 0.39

    (repetition tasks) to 1.20 (written language tasks), all fa-

    voring more intensive treatment.

    Clinical Question 5: For stroke-induced chronic apha-

    sia, what treatment outcomes are maintained following

    intensive language treatment? In the single-participant

    design of Raymer et al. (2006), ESs for picture-naming

    Cherney et al.:EBSR: Effects of Intensity of Treatment and CILT 1289

  • 8/11/2019 Intensidad de Terapia y Terapia Inducida Por Restriccin_ Control 2

    9/19

    Table 5. Intervention variables.

    Study Treatment program Treatment schedule Total amount Durat

    Basso & Caporali,2001 (Intensity)

    All participantsindividual treatment; conventionalaphasia treatment; varied techniques.

    More intensivetreatment

    NR All partic

    Goal areas= auditory comprehension, repetition,naming, and conversation.

    3 participants 5 hr/wk;Home program

    1440 m

    Treatment programmore intensive = clinic + homeprogram; less intensive = clinic.

    23 hr/dayLess intensive

    treatment3 participants;

    5 hr/wk

    Denes et al., 1996(Intensity)

    All participantsindividual treatment; ecological,based on stimulation approach to restore efficient useof language mainly in conversation.

    All participants Intensive treatmentgroup

    All partic

    Goal areas= auditory comprehension and verbalproduction.

    45- to 60-min sessionsM= 130 sessions,

    SD= 36,range = 94160

    M= 6 mrange = 5

    Technique= picture identification and conversation context.

    Intensive treatmentgroup

    Regular treatmentgroup

    67 sessions/wk

    M= 60 sessions,

    SD= 14,range = 5670

    Regular treatmentgroup

    About 3 sessions/wk

    Hinckley & Craig,1998, Study 3(Intensity)

    All participantsindividual, group and computer treatment;functionalist/pragmatic approach. Standard functionaltreatments (i.e., PACE and cueing hierarchy).

    Intensive treatmentphase

    All participants All partic

    23 hr/wk294306 hrs 18 wk

    Nonintensivetreatment phase

    6 wks intensivephase

    35 hr/wk6 wks nonintensive

    phase6 wks intensive

    phase

    Hinckley & Carr,2005 (Intensity)

    All participantscontext-based treatment that is basedon whole-task training and ecological validity.

    Intensive treatmentgroup

    Total number ofsessions per

    participant-NR

    NR

    Tasks= problem-solving catalogue ordering taskto develop compensatory strategies.

    20 hrs individualtreatment/wk; 5 hrgroup treatment/wk

    All participantsTechniques= role play, strategy development,

    and context-specific cues. Nonintensivetreatment group

    Minutes to reachcriterion on

    catalogue ordering taskTreatment groupintensive treatment = individual + group;nonintensive treatment = individual. 4 hr individual

    treatment/wkM= 233,

    range = 29597(110 sessions)

    (Continued on the following page)

    1290

    JournalofSpeech,

    Language,andHearingResearch

    Vol.51

    12821299

    October2008

  • 8/11/2019 Intensidad de Terapia y Terapia Inducida Por Restriccin_ Control 2

    10/19

    Table 5Continued. Intervention variables.

    Study Treatment program Treatment schedule Total amount Durati

    Maher et al.,2006 (CILT)

    All participantstask = primarily dual card task. All participants All participants All particTechnique= shaping and cueing. 3-hr sessions 24 hr 2 wkTreatment groupCILT = verbal response required. 4 sessions/wk PACE = all communication modalities permitted.

    Meinzer et al.,2004 (CILT)

    Treatment groupCIAT Verbal response required. Dualcard task (object drawings) 23 participants per group.

    All participants All participants All partic

    Technique= shaping and cueing. Model-based aphasiatreatment; deficit-specific approach.

    3 hr/day for 10 days 30 hr 2 wk

    NR = individual or group treatment.

    Meinzer et al.,2005 (CILT)

    Treatment groupCIAT = verbal response required; dual

    card task (object drawings); 23 participants per group.

    All participants All participants All particTechnique= shaping.3 hr/day for 10 days 30 hr 2 wkCIAT plus = CIAT as above, plus dual card task (object

    drawings, written words, and photographs); dailyhome program; daily communication practice withfamily members.

    Pulvermuller et al.,2001 (CILT andIntensity)

    Treatment group CIAT group CIAT group CIAT grCIAT = verbal response required; dual card task

    (object drawings); 23 participants per group.3 hr/day for 10 days M= 31.5 hr,

    range = 23332 wee

    Technique= Shaping and reinforcement.Conventional aphasia treatmentsyndrome-specific standard approach; naming, repetition,

    sentence completion, and conversation tasks.

    Conventionaltreatment group Conventional

    treatment group

    Conventtreatment

    35 wkSession length NRM = 33.9 hr,

    range = 2054

    35 wks

    (Continued on the following page)

    Cherney

    etal.:EBSR:EffectsofIntensityofTreatmentandCILT

    1291

  • 8/11/2019 Intensidad de Terapia y Terapia Inducida Por Restriccin_ Control 2

    11/19

    Table 5Continued. Intervention variables.

    Study Treatment program Treatment schedule Total amount Durati

    Pulvermuller et al.,2005 (CILT)

    All participantsCIAT; verbal response required; dual cardtask (object drawings); 3 participants per group.

    All participants All participants All partici

    Technique= shaping and reinforcement.3 hr/day for 10 days M= 31.3 hr,SD= 3.5,

    range = 23332 wk

    Raymer et al.,2006 (Intensity)

    All participantsMoss-talk multimode matching exercises;spoken and written word/picture matching.

    All participants All participants NRAlternating treatment

    schedules35 baseline sessions

    34 sessions/wk12 training sessions

    12 sessions/wk1-month break

    12 training sessions4 participants = 24 hr1 participant = 22 hr

    Note. NR = not reported; AAT = Aachen Aphasia Test (Huber, Poeck, & Weniger, 1984); ANT = Action Naming Test (Nicholas, Obler, Albert, & GGoodglass, & Weintraub, 2001); CADL = Communication Activities of Daily Living (Holland, Frattali, & Fromm, 1999); CAL = Communication Activity Effectiveness Index (Lomaset al.,1989);CU = content unit; PACE = Promoting Aphasics Communicative Effectiveness (Davis & Wilcox, 1985); PALPA

    Aphasia (Kay, Lesser, & Coltheart, 1992); TT = Token Test (De Renzi & Vignolo, 1962); WAB = Western Aphasia Battery (Kertesz, 1982).

    1292

    JournalofSpeech,

    Language,andHearingResearch

    Vol.51

    12821299

    October2008

  • 8/11/2019 Intensidad de Terapia y Terapia Inducida Por Restriccin_ Control 2

    12/19

    maintenance were 7.45 in the more intensive condi-

    tion and 4.85 in the less intensive condition. ESs for a

    word/picture verification task were 1.75 in the more

    intensive condition and 2.14 in the less intensive con-

    dition. Thus, maintenance effects of treatment inten-

    sity were mixed in this study.

    Stage of research and quality. Table 7 also shows the

    number of quality markers for each study and the stage

    of research.Of the two studies in the discovery phase of

    research, one received a quality marker score of 5 out of

    8 (Raymer et al., 2006) and the other received a qualitymarker score of 3 out of 8 (Basso & Caporali, 2001). Three

    studies were efficacy studies (Denes et al., 1996; Hinckley

    & Carr, 2005; Pulvermuller et al., 2001), each of which

    attained a quality marker score of 6 or 7 out of 9. One

    study (Hinckley & Craig, 1998) fell into the effectiveness

    stage of research, achieving a quality score of 4 out of 8.

    CILT Results

    CILT was examined in five studies as shown in

    Table 8. These studies addressed Questions 6, 7, and 10.

    No studies of CILT in acute aphasia were available, so

    Questions 8 and 9 could not be addressed. Cohens dval-

    ues could be calculated for all five CILT studies. The ESs

    for Meinzer et al. (2004, 2005), and Pulvermuller et al.

    (2005) represent within-subject effects, whereas those in

    Maher et al. (2006) and Pulvermuller et al. (2001) come

    from between-group comparisons of CILT witha contrast-

    ing treatment.

    Clinical Question 6: For stroke-induced chronic apha-

    sia, what is the influence of constraint-induced language

    therapy on measures of language impairment?Across thefive group studies, 16 ESs were calculable, including 9

    large ESs in favor of CILT. In individuals with chronic

    aphasia, CILT hadpositive effects for overall aphasia bat-

    tery scores (Maher et al., 2006; Meinzer et al., 2004) and

    subtests of auditory comprehension, word retrieval, repe-

    tition, and lexical decision (Maher et al., 2006; Meinzer

    et al., 2004; Pulvermuller et al., 2005).

    Meinzer et al. (2005) examined a modification of

    CILT (described as CIAT) in which additional home ac-

    tivities were included. There was no difference between

    CILT and CIAT plus on outcome scores immediately

    Table 6.Study quality marker variables.

    Study DesignAssessorblinding

    Randomsamplingdescribed

    Participantscomparable/

    describedTreatment

    fidelity reportedValid outcome

    measure SignificanceIntent-to-treat Precision

    Basso & Caporali,2001 (Intensity)

    Case study No No Yes No Yes Yes N/A No

    Denes et al., 1996(Intensity)

    Controlledtrial

    Yes No Yes No Yes Yes Yes Yes

    Hinckley & Craig,1998, Study 3(Intensity)

    Case series No No Yes No Yes Yes N/A Yes

    Hinckley & Carr,2005 (Intensity)

    Controlledtrial

    No No Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes

    Maher et al.,2006 (CILT)

    Controlledtrial

    No No Yes Yes Yes Yes No Yes

    Meinzer et al.,2004 (CILT)

    Case series No No Yes No Yes Yes N/A Yes

    Meinzer et al.,2005 (CILT)

    Controlledtrial

    No No Yes No Yes Yes Yes No

    Pulvermuller et al.,2001 (CILTand Intensity)

    Controlledtrial

    Yes Yes Yes No Yes Yes No No

    Pulvermuller et al.,2005 (CILT)

    Case series No No Yes No Yes Yes N/A Yes

    Raymer et al.,2006 (Intensity)

    Single subject No No Yes No Yes Yes N/A Yes

    Note. N/A = not applicable.

    Cherney et al.:EBSR: Effects of Intensity of Treatment and CILT 1293

  • 8/11/2019 Intensidad de Terapia y Terapia Inducida Por Restriccin_ Control 2

    13/19

    following training. Both groups reported significant im-

    provements. For language impairment measures, CILT

    consistently led to positive outcomes.

    Clinical Question 7: For stroke-induced chronic

    aphasia, what is the influence of constraint-induced lan-

    guage therapy on measures of communication activity/

    participation?Three of the 5 CILT studies included mea-

    sures of communication activity/participation; ESs could

    be calculated for 11 outcome measures incorporated in the

    studies (e.g., Communicative Activity Log, storyretelling,

    Communicative Effectiveness Index; Lomas et al., 1989).

    ESs ranged from.82 to 3.77, with eight favoring CILT (six

    large effects) and three favoring a comparison treatment

    (one large effect). Thus, CILT had positive outcomes for

    several measures of communication activity/participation,

    although some favored a comparison condition.

    ClinicalQuestion 10: For stroke-induced chronic apha-

    sia, what treatment outcomes are maintained following

    Table 7. Intensity studies, effect sizes, and methodologic quality.

    Study Outcome measure(s) ICF Intensity effect sizeResearch

    stageQuality

    scoreClinical

    question

    Basso & Caporali, 2001 AAT TT I Not calculable Discovery 3 of 8 1, 2Ravens matricesa IPicture description I

    Denes et al., 1996 AAT: TT I 0.60 Efficacy 7 of 9 3Repetition I 0.39Written Language I 1.20Naming I 0.73Comprehension I 0.91Profile Level I 0.83

    Hinckley & Craig, 1998, Study 3 Intensive Treatment I vs. Nonintensive Effectiveness 4 of 8 1, 2BNT I 1.12Content Unit Analysis A/P 0.53

    Intensive Treatment II vs. NonintensiveBNT I 0.95Content Unit Analysis A/P 0.74

    Hinckley & Carr, 2005 Catalogue ordering Efficacy 7 of 9 1,2Oral (quiet) A/P 0.81Oral (concurrent) A/P 0.05Written (quiet) A/P 0.54Written (concurrent) A/P 0.18

    CADL-2 A/P 1.15PALPA Oral Naming I 0.16PALPA Written Naming I 1.48

    Pulvermuller et al., 2001 AAT: Overall Test Scores I 2.18 Efficacy 6 of 9 1, 2TT I 0.92Naming I 1.12Language Comprehension I 1.12

    Repetition I Not calculableCommunicative Activi ty Log: Patients A/P 3.78SLPs A/P 2.64

    Raymer et al., 2006 WAB Aphasia Quotient I Not calculable Discovery 5 of 8 1, 5BNT I Not calculablePicture Naming Acquisition I 4.35 (lw), 11.37 (hw)Picture Naming Maintenance I 4.85 (lw), 7.45 (hw)Word/Picture Verification Acquisition I 2.72 (lw), 2.14 (hw)Word/Picture Verification Maintenance I 2.14 (lw), 1.75 (hw)

    Note. lw = low weighted intensity; hw = high weighted intensity; ICF = International Classification System; I = impairment; A/P = activity/participationSLP = speech-language pathologist.aRavens Coloured Progressive Matrices (Raven, Court, & Raven, 1986)

    1294 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol. 51 12821299 October 2008

  • 8/11/2019 Intensidad de Terapia y Terapia Inducida Por Restriccin_ Control 2

    14/19

    constraint-induced language therapy? Two studies ad-

    dressed Question 10. Meinzer et al. (2005) reported that

    effects of CILT on measures of language impairment

    and communication activity/participation were main-

    tained at 6 months compared with baseline performance.Family members of the group receiving CIATplus with

    the modified home program reported further gains in

    communication effectiveness over the 6-month period.

    Maher et al. (2006)reported follow-up testing at 1 month

    post-CILT completion. Three of 4 CILT participants and

    1 of 3 participants in Promoting Aphasics Communica-

    tive Effectiveness (PACE) demonstrated continued in-

    creases on aphasia tests over the 1-month period.

    Stage of research and quality.Table 8 indicates that

    two studies that fell into the discovery phase of re-

    search received quality scores of 4 out of 8. Three studies

    represented efficacy research, two of which received a

    quality marker score of 6 out of9 and one of which received

    a score of 5 out of 9.

    DiscussionThe purpose of this EBSR was to assess the in-

    fluence of intensity of language treatment and CILT on

    language and communication outcomes of individuals

    with stroke-induced aphasia. A systematic search of the

    literature from 1990 to 2006 yielded only 10 studies that

    met predetermined inclusion criteria, with 5 studies ad-

    dressing treatment intensity, 4 studies addressing CILT,

    and 1 study addressing both. Although few studies were

    expected to be found for CILTa technique that was

    Table 8.CILT studies, effect sizes, and methodologic quality.

    Study Outcome measure(s) ICF CILT effect size Research stage Quality score Clinical question

    Maher et al., 2006 Aphasia tests Efficacy 6 of 9 6, 7, 10WAB Aphasia Quotient I 1.01BNT I 0.16Action Naming Test I 0.14

    Linguistic measures in story retellingNumber of words A/P 0.72Number of utterances A/P 0.82Number of sentences A/P 0.19Mean length of utterance A/P 0.33

    Clinician ratings A/P Not calculable

    Meinzer et al., 2004 AAT: Profile I 0.34 Discovery 4 of 8 6Token Test I 0.81

    Meinzer et al., 2005 AAT: Profile I 1.63 Efficacy 5 of 9 6, 7, 10Subtests I Not calculable

    CETI: Overall relatives A/P 1.86Communication Activity Log

    Quantity: Patients A/P 1.99Relatives A/P 2.35

    Comprehension: Patients A/P .47 Relatives A/P 1.13

    Pulvermuller et al., 2001 AAT: Overall Test Scores I 2.18 Efficacy 6 of 9 6, 7 TT I 0.92Naming I 1.12Language Comprehension I 1.12Repetition I Not calculable

    Communicative Activity Log: Patients A/P 3.77SLPs A/P 2.64

    Pulvermuller et al., 2005 AAT: TT I 0.25 Discovery 4 of 8 6

    Repetition I 0.11Naming I 0.25Comprehension I 0.46

    Lexical decision reaction timesResponse time I 2.39Pseudowords x time I 3.32Words I Not calculable

    Cherney et al.:EBSR: Effects of Intensity of Treatment and CILT 1295

  • 8/11/2019 Intensidad de Terapia y Terapia Inducida Por Restriccin_ Control 2

    15/19

    introduced only in 2001the paucity of studies directly

    addressing treatmentintensity was surprising in view of

    the overall large number of studies examining language

    treatment for stroke-induced aphasia. Given the small

    number of studies, the conclusions of the present sys-

    tematic review must be considered preliminary. Further-

    more, interpretation of ESs and their applicability to the

    aphasia literature is not certain at this time. Neverthe-less, certain trends in the literature from 1990 to 2006

    may usefully inform both clinical practice and future re-

    search in this area.

    Increased treatment intensity was associated with

    positive changes in outcome measures of language im-

    pairment in 68 persons with chronic and acute aphasia.

    All ESs calculated for group comparisons on language

    impairment measures favored more intensive treatment

    over less intensive treatment, including seven large ef-

    fect sizes. In the single-participant study (Raymer et al.,

    2006), ESsfor one measure favored more intensive treat-

    ment and one favored less intensive. The data for Clin-ical Question 2 addressing change in communication

    activity/participation were mixed, with some favoring

    more intensive treatment and some favoring less inten-

    sive treatment for persons with chronic aphasia. These

    observations suggest that there can be complex interac-

    tions among intensity of treatment schedule, type of

    treatment, and type of outcome measure.

    Maintenance of treatment intensity effects were

    reported in one study of persons with chronic aphasia

    (Raymer et al., 2006). Findings also were equivocal, fa-

    voring more intensive treatment for one outcome mea-

    sure and less intensive treatment for the other. However,there was only one data point for each maintenance ob-

    servation. Beeson and Robey (2006) suggest a minimum

    of two data points for calculation of an ES. Therefore, this

    result should be taken with caution.

    Overall, five studies involving 90 participants

    reported that CILT resulted in positive changes on mea-

    sures of language impairment and communication activity/

    participation in individuals with chronic aphasia, in-

    cluding large ESs for 9 of 16 impairment measures and

    6 of 11 activity/participation measures. Meinzer et al.

    (2005) reported that effects were maintained for as long

    as 6 months following completion of CILT in 27 patients.No data addressed the effect of CILT in patients with

    acute aphasia, however. Finally, it is important to note

    that Maher et al. (2006) also found significant improve-

    ments on language measures for participants in their

    control group, who participated in an intensive version

    of PACE in which use of compensatory modalities is al-

    lowed. This finding implies that intensity is a key com-

    ponent of CILT.

    Interpretation of the results of this EBSR and its

    implications for clinical practice must be considered in

    conjunction with the information about characteristics

    of the participants. For the intensity studies, the major-

    ity of participants had nonfluent forms of aphasia. When

    severity of aphasia was provided, the majority of partic-

    ipants tended to have more severe aphasia, with many

    described as having a global aphasia. Therefore, conclu-

    sions regarding intensity of treatment are most applica-

    ble to individuals with severe nonfluent aphasia. Futureresearch needs to address, in particular, the impact of

    treatment intensity for participants presenting with a

    milder aphasia as well as participants who are catego-

    rized as fluent. Similarly for the CILT studies, the ma-

    jority of participants were nonfluent and moderately

    impaired, thereby limiting the generalizability of the re-

    sults in individuals with fluent aphasia and individuals

    with mild and severe aphasia. Chronicity of aphasia is

    another participant characteristic that requires atten-

    tion in future studies. No study of CILT included in-

    dividuals with acute aphasia, and only one treatment

    intensity study addressed the impact of intensity onacute aphasia.

    One of the difficulties encountered in this system-

    atic review was that of comparing results across studies

    when there were differences in the outcome measures

    used. Therefore, we attempted to find commonalities

    by categorizing the types of outcome measures accord-

    ing to the WHO (2001) ICFlevels of body function (lan-

    guage reception and production impairment) and activity/

    participation. Overall, all of the studies included at least

    one outcome measure at the language impairment level

    and most of these measures were standardized, valid as-

    sessment instruments. Although all of the CILT studiesand four of the six intensity studies included at least

    one measure at the communication activity/participation

    level, there were fewer of these measures, they were typ-

    ically individualized, and information on their validity

    and reliability was often lacking. Future studies of CILT

    and clinical practice should consider inclusion of more

    measures at the communication activity/participation

    level as well as studies that address quality of life in

    patients with aphasia.

    The variety of primary and secondary outcome mea-

    sures made comparisons across studies difficult and com-

    plicated the determination of the validity and reliabilityas they contribute to the quality of the study. Therefore,

    our critical appraisal used a quality marker related to

    outcome measuresthat represented only a minimal level

    of quality in these areas. All studies achieved this min-

    imal level of quality that related to use of a valid and

    reliable primary outcome measure.

    A number of methodological weaknesses in the 10

    studies included in the EBSR were identified during the

    critical appraisal of the quality of each study. The short-

    comings across the studies related to primarily three

    1296 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol. 51 12821299 October 2008

  • 8/11/2019 Intensidad de Terapia y Terapia Inducida Por Restriccin_ Control 2

    16/19

    quality indicators: random assignment of subjects, as-

    sessor blinding, and evidence of treatment fidelity. In

    most of the studies, regardless of stage of research and

    research design, participants were selected as a conve-

    nience sample. Randomization is critical for future re-

    search, as many evidence-based review organizations

    (e.g., The Cochrane Collaboration) include primarily ran-

    domized trials in their systematic reviews of health careinterventions. It is also essential that investigators doc-

    ument procedures to ensure fidelity of the actual treat-

    ment techniques and that some measure of procedural

    reliability is provided. Clear documentation of the treat-

    ment technique will facilitate the transference of the tech-

    nique to the clinical setting.

    Examination of the studies in relation to stage of re-

    search clearly shows where the strengths of the current

    research lie and the direction of future research. In-

    cluded studies in the present EBSR were three efficacy

    studies evaluating the impact of intensity on stroke-

    related chronic aphasia and three efficacy studies eval-uating the impact of CILT on chronic aphasia. Only one

    intensity study evaluated effectiveness, and no study

    evaluated effectiveness of CILT. No studies in either cat-

    egory assessed policy change or cost effectiveness. There-

    fore, as evidence for efficacy of more intensive treatment

    and CILT continues to grow, future research must ad-

    dress issues of effectiveness and cost effectiveness with

    studies that are designed according to the criteria in-

    cluded in the ASHA levels-of-evidence scheme to ensure

    that the studies are of high quality.

    Regardless of type of treatment, our general con-

    clusions indicate that more treatment is better over a re-stricted time interval. No studies have addressed what

    might be the optimum amount and intensity of treat-

    ment; however, it is possible that no single answer exists

    and that optimum dosage of high-intensity treatment

    varies depending on the type of treatment and the char-

    acteristics of the participants aphasia, including chro-

    nicity. Future studies, beginning at the discovery phase,

    are needed to address the many confounds inherent in

    resolving the issue of optimum intensity of treatment for

    individuals with stroke-induced aphasia.

    Although this systematic review has highlighted the

    potential efficacy of CILT on the basis of its dual prin-ciples, future studies need to be designed with the aim of

    teasing out theimpact of constraint and intensity on out-

    come. This task is particularly difficult because high in-

    tensity is such an integral part of CILT. However Maher

    et al. (2006) have demonstrated that it is possible to ad-

    dress this issue methodologically by providing an iden-

    tical schedule of CILT and its control treatment. Although

    results suggest that efficacy is related to the nature of the

    treatment (i.e., constraint) rather than to intensity alone,

    given the small number of participants, results are not

    yet conclusive. More definitive evidence will be derived

    only from (a) conducting randomized studies comparing

    CILT with other treatment procedures given at a simi-

    lar intensity schedule using participants of similar type

    and severity of aphasia and (b) measuring outcomes at

    the levels of language impairment and communication

    activity/participation. Additionally, the optimum inten-

    sity of treatment for CILT has yet to be determined. Fur-ther studies comparing constraints at different levels of

    intensity are needed.

    Although the early evidence surrounding the efficacy

    of CILT for individuals with aphasia shows some promise,

    the effects are similar to other intensive treatments that

    do not employ the use of constraint. Therefore, these re-

    sults should be taken under advisement. Clear and con-

    vincing evidence supporting the effectiveness of CILT

    over other aphasia treatments has not yet been estab-

    lished, and further research comparing aphasia treat-

    ments is warranted. Clinicians should interpret the

    findings from this EBSR conservatively and always inconjunction with their patients unique characteristics,

    circumstances, and preferences when considering the

    best course of treatment for and with their clients.

    Finally, some observations need to be made following

    this pilot implementation of ASHAs levels-of-evidence

    scheme. The indicators used to judge the quality of re-

    search studies tend to focus on issues that are most per-

    tinent to group study designs. Therefore, the usefulness

    of the levels-of-evidence scheme in evaluating single-

    participant research designs, which are used commonly

    in the speech-languagepathology literature, is in need of

    further development. Although eight of the nine indica-tors were applicable to single-participant design research,

    other quality indicators related to the documentation of

    experimental control and threats to external validity (e.g.,

    stabilityof baselinemeasures) that areunique to this type

    of research were not included. These additional issues of

    design quality that arise in single-participant research

    will need to be studied.

    Another notable aspect of the current systematic re-

    view is that all studies addressing the clinical questions,

    including discovery studies, are included. Although

    some systematic reviews include only group designs or

    randomized clinical trials, we elected to include all stud-ies, regardless of design. Ultimately, we need to give

    greater weight to well-designed randomized trials. How-

    ever, given the relative paucity of those studies in our

    current literature base, the inclusion of all possible stud-

    ies gives a broader perspective of thestate of knowledge.

    It is our hope that this systematic review will incite ad-

    ditional rigorous clinical trials addressing the questions

    of intensity of aphasia treatment and CILT, as well as

    other areas of interest to the fields of speech-language

    pathology and audiology.

    Cherney et al.:EBSR: Effects of Intensity of Treatment and CILT 1297

  • 8/11/2019 Intensidad de Terapia y Terapia Inducida Por Restriccin_ Control 2

    17/19

    Acknowledgments

    This EBSR was supported by ASHAs National Center

    for Evidence-Based Practice in Communication Disorders

    (N-CEP), the Advisory Committee on Evidence-Based Practice

    in Communication Disorders, and ASHAs Special Interest

    Division 2: Neurophysiology and Neurogenic Speech and

    Language Disorders. We thank the following individuals who

    contributed to the preparation of this article: Rob Mullen,N-CEP Director; Beverly Wang, N-CEP Information Manager;

    and Floyd Roye, N-CEP Project Administrator. All members

    of this evidence-based review panel agreed to declare no

    competing interests in relation to this article. No author had

    any paid consultancy or any other conflict of interest with this

    document. The first three authors contributed equally to the

    preparation of this article, and the order does not reflect any

    differences in contribution.

    References

    ASHA.(2001).Scope of practice in speech-language pathology

    [Scope of Practice]. Available from www.asha.org/policy.

    Basso, A. A., & Caporali, A. (2001). Aphasia therapy or theimportance of being earnest. Aphasiology, 15,307332.

    Beeson, P., & Robey, R. (2006). Evaluating single-subjecttreatment research: Lessons learned from the aphasialiterature.Neuropsychological Review, 16,161169.

    Bhogal,S. K., Teasell,R., & Speechley, M. (2003). Intensityof aphasia therapy, impact on recovery.Stroke, 34,987993.

    Busk, P. L., & Serlin, R. (1992).Meta-analysis for single caseresearch. In T. R. Kratochwill & J. R. Levin (Eds.), Single-case research design and analysis: New directions for psy-chology and education. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral

    sciences(2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Davis, G. A., & Wilcox, M. (1985).Adult aphasia rehabilita-tion: Applied pragmatics. San Diego, CA: College Hill Press.

    Denes, G., Perazzolo, C., Piani, A., & Piccione, F. (1996).Intensive versus regular speech therapy in global aphasia:

    A controlled study.Aphasiology, 10, 385394.

    De Renzi, E., & Vignolo, L. (1962). The Token Test: Asensitive test to detect receptive disturbances in aphasics.

    Brain, 85, 665678.

    Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1983).The Boston DiagnosticAphasia Examination. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.

    Hakkennes, S., & Keating, J. (2005). Constraint-induced

    movement therapy following stroke: A systematic review ofrandomized controlled trials. Australian Journal of Physio-therapy, 51,221231.

    Hedges, L. V., & Olkin,I. (1985).Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

    Hinckley, J. J., & Craig, H. K. (1998). Influence of rate oftreatment on the naming abilities of adults with chronicaphasia. Aphasiology, 12,9891006.

    Hinckley, J. J., & Carr, T. (2005). Comparing the outcomes ofintensive and non-intensive context-based aphasia treat-ment.Aphasiology, 19,965974.

    Holland, A., Frattali, C., & Fromm, D. (1999). Communica-tion activities of daily living (2nd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Hollingshead, A. B. (1975).Four Factor Index of SocialStatus. New Haven, CT: Yale University.

    Huber, H. P., Poeck, K., & Williams, K.(1984). The AachenAphasia Test. In F. C. Rose (Ed.), Progress in aphasiology(pp. 291303). New York: Raven Press.

    Huber, W., Poeck, K., & Weniger, D. (1984). The AachenAphasia Test.In F. C. Rose (Ed.),Advances in neurology, Vol. 42:Progress in aphasiology. New York: Raven Press.

    Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (2001).TheBoston Naming Test. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.

    Kay, J., Lesser, R., & Coltheart, M. (1992). Psycholinguisticassessments of language processing in aphasia. Hove, UnitedKingdom: Psychology Press.

    Kertesz, A. (1982). Western Aphasia Battery. Orlando, FL:Grune & Stratton.

    Kleim, J. A., & Jones, T. A. (2008). Principles of experience-dependent neural plasticity: Implications for rehabilitationafter brain damage.Journal of Speech-Language-Hearing

    Research, 51(Suppl.), S225S239.

    Lomas, J., Pickard, L., Bester, S., Elbard, H., Finlayson,A., & Zoghaib, C. (1989). The Communicative Effective-ness Index: Development and psychometric evaluation ofa functional communication measure for adult aphasia.

    Journal of Speech Hearing Disorders, 54,113124.

    Maher, L., Kendall, D., Swearengin, J., Rodriguez, A.,Leon, S., Pingel, K., Holland, A., & Rothi, L. (2006).

    A pilot study of use-dependent learning in the context ofconstraint induced language therapy. Journal of the Inter-national Neuropsychological Society, 12,843852.

    Meinzer, M., Djundja, D., Barthel, G., Elbert, T., &Rockstroth, B.(2005). Long-term stability of improvedlanguage functions in chronic aphasia after constraint-induced aphasia therapy.Stroke, 36, 14621466.

    Meinzer, M., Elbert, C., Wienbruch, C., Djundja, D.,

    Barthel, G., & Rockstroh, B.(2004). Intensive languagetraining enhances brain plasticity in chronic aphasia. BMC

    Biology, 2,19.

    Nicholas, M., Obler, L., Albert, M., & Goodglass, H. (1985).Lexical retrieval in healthy aging.Cortex, 21, 595606.

    Poek, K., Huber, W., & Willmes, K.(1989). Outcomes ofintensive language treatment in aphasia.Journal of Speechand Hearing Disorders, 54, 471479.

    Pulvermuller, F. B., Neininger, B., Elbert, T., Mohr, B.,Rockstroh, B., Koebbel, P., & Taub, E. (2001). Constraint-induced therapy of chronic aphasia after stroke.Stroke, 32,16211626.

    Pulvermuller, F., Hauk, O., Zohsel, K., Neininger, B., &

    Mohr, B. (2005). Therapy-related reorganization of lan-guage in both hemispheres of patients with chronic aphasiaNeuroimage, 28,481489.

    Raymer, A., Kohen, F., & Saffell, D. (2006). Computerisedtraining for impairments for word comprehension andretrieval in aphasia.Aphasiology, 20, 257268.

    Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1986). RavensColoured Progressive Matrices. London: H. K. Lewis & Co.

    Robey, R. R.(1998). A meta-analysis of clinical outcomes inthe treatment of aphasia.Journal of Speech, Language, and

    Hearing Research, 41, 172187.

    Robey, R. R.(2004). A five-phase model for clinical-outcomesresearch.Journal of Communication Disorders, 5, 401411.

    1298 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol. 51 12821299 October 2008

  • 8/11/2019 Intensidad de Terapia y Terapia Inducida Por Restriccin_ Control 2

    18/19

    Taub, E.(1977). Movement in nonhuman primates deprivedof somatosensory feedback.Exercise and Sports SciencesReview, 4,335374.

    Taub, E., Miller, N., Novack, T., & Cook, E. (1993). Tech-nique to improve chronic motor deficit after stroke. Archivesof Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 74, 347354.

    Taub, E., Uswatte, G., King, D. K., Morris, D. M., Crago,J. E., & Chatterjee, A.(2006). A placebo controlled trial of

    constraint-induced movement therapy for upper extremityafter stroke. Stroke, 37, 10451049.

    Taub, E., Uswatte, G., & Piclikiti, R. (1999). Constraint-induced movement therapy: A new family of techniques withbroad application to physical rehabilitationA clinicalreview.Journal of Rehabilitation Research and Develop-ment, 36,237251.

    Taub, E., & Wolf, S. (1997). Constraint-induced movementtechniques to facilitate upper extremity use in strokepatients.Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 3, 3861.

    Wolf, S., Winstein, C. J., Miller, J., Taub, E., Uswatte, G.,Morris, D., et al. (2006). Effect of constraint-induced move-ment therapy on upper extremity function 3 to 9 monthsafter stroke: The EXCITE randomized clinical trial. Jour-nal of the American Medical Association, 296, 20953104.

    World Health Organization. (2001).International Classifi-cation of Functioning, Disability and Health: ICF. Geneva,Switzerland: Author.

    Received September 4, 2007

    Accepted February 24, 2008

    DOI: 10.1044/1092-4388(2008/07-0206)

    Contact author: Tobi Frymark, National Center for Evidence-

    Based Practice in Communication Disorders, American

    Speech-Language-Hearing Association, 2200 Research

    Boulevard, Rockville, MD 20850. E-mail: [email protected].

    Cherney et al.:EBSR: Effects of Intensity of Treatment and CILT 1299

  • 8/11/2019 Intensidad de Terapia y Terapia Inducida Por Restriccin_ Control 2

    19/19


Recommended